Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
http://www.baillement.com

mystery of yawning 

 

 

mise à jour du
19 septembre 2021
Sci Rep.
2021;11(1):17957
 
First evidence of yawn contagion
in a wild monkey species
Gallo A, Zanoli A, Caselli M, Palagi E, Norscia I.

Chat-logomini

 
Tous les articles sur la contagion du bâillement
 
All articles about contagious yawning
 
Yawn contagion occurs when individuals yawn in response to the yawn of others (triggers). This is the first account of yawn contagion in wild geladas (Theropithecus gelada), a monkey species that shows yawn contagion in captivity and is organized in core units (one-male/bachelor groups) forming multilevel associations. In a population of geladas from the Kundi plateau (Ethiopia) the authors found that the yawning response was highest when geladas could perceive a triggering yawn, which confirms that yawn contagion is present in the wild. Yawn duration, mouth-opening degree and presence/absence of vocalisation (possibly modulating yawn detectability) did not affect the likelihood of contagion. Males and females, known to be both implicated in movement initiation within groups, were similarly powerful as yawn triggers. Instead, group membership and responder sex had a significant role in shaping the phenomenon. Yawn contagion was highest between individuals belonging to different core units and males were most likely to respond to others' yawns. Because males have a non-negligible role in inter-group coordination, these results suggest that yawn contagion may have a communicative function that goes beyond the basic unit level.
 
 
La contagion du bâillement se produit lorsque des individus bâillent en réponse au bâillement des autres (déclencheurs). Il s'agit du premier constat de la contagion du bâillement chez les Geladas sauvages (Theropithecus gelada), une espèce de singe qui présente une contagion du bâillement en captivité et est organisée en groupes hiérarchisés (groupes d'un seul mâle/célibataire) formant des associations à plusieurs niveaux. Dans une population de géladas du plateau de Kundi (Éthiopie), les auteurs ont constaté que la réponse du bâillement était la plus élevée lorsque les géladas pouvaient percevoir un bâillement déclencheur, ce qui confirme que la contagion du bâillement est présente dans la nature. La durée du bâillement, le degré d'ouverture de la bouche et la présence/absence de vocalisation (éventuellement en modulant la détectabilité du bâillement) n'ont pas affecté la probabilité de contagion. Les mâles et les femelles, connus pour être tous deux impliqués dans l'initiation du mouvement au sein des groupes, avaient la même faculté pour déclencher des bâillements. Au lieu de cela, l'appartenance à un groupe et le sexe des répondants jouent un rôle important dans la réplication. La contagion du bâillement était la plus fréquente entre les individus appartenant à différents groupes de base et les mâles étaient les plus susceptibles de réagir aux bâillements des autres. Parce que les mâles ont un rôle non négligeable dans la coordination intergroupe, ces résultats suggèrent que la contagion du bâillement peut avoir une fonction de communication qui dépasse le niveau du groupe de base.
 
Introduction
 
Contagious yawning differs from spontaneous yawning in that one individual (responder) yawns in response to a yawn emitted by another individual (trigger)1. In this respect, the yawn of an individual acts as a triggering stimulus (sensu Tinbergen2). While spontaneous yawning is probably an ancient phenomenon widespread in vertebrates3,4, intraspecific contagious yawning is an evolutionary more recent phenomenon that has been found mostly in highly social species. Such species include mammals (domestic pig, Sus scrofa5; sheep, Ovis aries6, lions, Panthera leo7, elephant seals, Mirounga leonina8; Tonkean macaques, Macaca tonkeana9; bonobos, Pan paniscus10; chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes11,12; humans, Homo sapiens13; but see: Pongo pygmaeus14) and one bird species (budgerigars, Melopsittacus undulates15). Moreover, there are some social species in which yawn contagion is virtually absent (e.g. lemurs, Lemur catta and Varecia variegata16; lowland gorillas, Gorilla gorilla gorilla17). In different species, yawn contagion can be triggered both by vocalized and non-vocalized yawns18,19, regardless of the uni- or multi-modal nature of the stimulus13,18,20. Yawn contagion can be influenced by environmental/social contexts and individual factors, and it can follow a diurnal rhythm with a peak in the morning21,22. Moreover, the phenomenon has been found to be highest between familiar or in-group subjects (wolves23; bonobos24; chimpanzees12; humans18,25). Finally, depending on the social role played by each sex in social groups, the yawning response can increase when triggers are males (dominant in chimpanzees11) or females (dominant in bonobos26). In humans, women may respond more to others' yawns27,28, although this does not apply to all cohorts18,29. Overall, yawn contagion seems to promote inter-individual physiological resonance30,31, synchronisation within social groups7 and coordinated daily activities7,32.
 
Geladas (Theropithecus gelada) are a diurnal monkey species, endemic to Ethiopia, that lives in core units (One-Male-Unit, OMU; All-Male-Unit, AMU) forming discrete multilevel associations (teams and bands33). The core units are characterized by high levels of grooming, low levels of aggression, and no coercive control by males over females, with females mediating group coordination with the alpha male34. While grooming is observed only within groups and never between groups33, high levels of tolerance characterize the groups forming the band, which show low aggressive levels when they occupy the same foraging areas33.
 
Geladas have been the first monkey species in which yawn contagion was demonstrated35. In captive geladas yawn contagion was elicited by all types of yawns (covered teeth, uncovered teeth and uncovered gums) emitted with or without vocalizations. Moreover, it was highest between subjects sharing strong bonds (measured via grooming exchange rates)35. So far, no study has described the phenomenon of yawn contagion in geladas, and more generally in primates, under wild conditions.
 
Here, we investigated yawn contagion in a population of wild geladas to verify whether the phenomenon was present in the natural setting. Moreover, we investigated the possible influence of the factors that are only assessable under wild conditions to understand if the phenomenon can act on a multilayer scale reflecting the multilevel social organization of the species. Based on the previous framework, we formulated the following predictions.
 
Prediction 1&emdash;Presence of yawn contagion. As previously reported in captivity35, we expected to find that individuals would be more likely to yawn when they perceived the yawn of a conspecific compared to when they did not (presence of yawn contagion).
 
Prediction 2&emdash;Perceptual factors. In line with the findings obtained in captivity35, we expected that also in the wild yawn contagion would be induced at comparable levels by different types of triggering yawns and regardless of the presence of vocalization.
 
Prediction 3&emdash;Contextual and individual factors. As it occurs in humans (characterized by a diurnal lifestyle) that show a peak of yawn contagion in the morning22, we expected that in geladas yawn contagion could be highest in the first part of the day (Prediction 3a). Because in primates, including captive geladas, yawn contagion has been found to be most frequent between familiar subjects12,18,23,35, in geladas we expected to find a higher rate of contagion within rather than between groups (Prediction 3b). Finally, the different role of each sex in managing social groups can lead to sex differences in yawn contagion25&endash;27. Because in geladas both males and females play an important role in managing the social groups34, we expected to find no difference in yawn contagion rates between sexes (Prediction 3c).
 
Discussion
 
The present study provides the first evidence of yawn contagion in a wild primate, the gelada (in line with Prediction 1). In the considered time window, the study subjects were more likely to yawn when they could perceive a yawn from conspecifics compared to when they could not (Fig. 2b; Table _Table1).1). Most of the yawning responses occurred in the second minute, thus confirming captive data35. The physical distance between trigger and responder, yawn duration, and trigger yawn type (accompanied or not by vocalization) had no effect on the occurrence of the phenomenon (Table _(Table1).1). Hence, the level of yawn detectability (possibly enhanced in case of long, vocalized, and wide-open yawns) appeared not to be strictly linked to the yawning response probability (in agreement with Prediction 2). This is in line with the previous report showing that all yawn types can be contagious in geladas35. Our data are also in agreement with results on humans that are infected by others' yawns by having access to both total or partial view of the trigger's face18,20,41. Palagi et al.35 found a matching of the yawn type especially in the contagion between females. We could not check for this aspect due to data limitation, but it is certainly an issue that would be worth exploring in future investigations.
 
As in captivity35, also in the wild both vocalised and non-vocalised yawns could elicit contagion at a comparable level (Table _(Table1).1). This result is also consistent with findings on humans, where contagion is triggered by both the visual and acoustic components of the stimulus19,25,47. The acoustic component of the yawn might function when geladas are located far from the trigger and cannot visually perceive yawning. Further investigation including larger observational ranges and datasets per individual is necessary to clarify the role of multimodality on this phenomenon.
 
Contrary to our prediction (3b), in the study population yawn contagion was highest between individuals belonging to different groups (Fig. 2c, Table _Table1).1). Although the adult members of different groups do not engage in affiliative behaviours, they show high tolerance levels, spatial cohesion, and collective moving33. Considering the complexity of the gelada multilevel social system48, yawn contagion could be a phenomenon acting on a multilayer scale. Our limited dataset is silent on whether yawn contagion is affected by the affiliative relationships (e.g., grooming) shared by group members, thus not allowing the verification of this possibility, already tested in captivity35.
 
Contrary to the expectation (Prediction 3a), in the Kundi population the time of the day had no effect on contagion (Table _(Table1).1). Because geladas arrived on the plateau relatively late in the morning (9&endash;10 am), it was not possible to sample their behaviour soon after they started their activities. Expanding data collection to different contexts, including sleeping sites, would be crucial to determine how yawn contagion is distributed within groups according to the social affiliation shared by subjects over the 24-h cycle. However, reaching out our geladas to sample their behaviour at such sites on the cliffs may be challenging.
 
Finally, we found that yawn contagion: (i) was triggered to a similar extent by males and females, and (ii) males most frequently responded to others' yawns (Fig. 2d, Table _Table1,1, Prediction 3c not supported). In the wild controlling for all the potential arousal stimuli may not be completely possible. However, our conservative exclusion of possible perturbing events (aggression, human disturbance, and copulations) and the fact that males did not preferentially respond with uncovered teeth yawns (often associated with social tension and agonistic contexts in males38) makes it unlikely that the response by males could be stress driven.
 
The trend of yawn contagion between sexes and groups suggests that this phenomenon may have a communicative function, related to activity coordination. Males and females, which were similarly powerful as yawn triggers (Table _(Table1), 1), can be both implicated in movement initiation within groups34. However, males frequently initiate the group movement, followed by the dominant female and the rest of the groups34. Hence, the fact that males automatically responded more frequently to others' yawns and that yawn contagion was highest between groups, suggests that in the wild yawn contagion might also favour inter-group synchronisation during daily activities. The Social Communicative Hypothesis predicts that in different contexts&emdash;through yawning&emdash;individuals can involuntarily convey information about their internal state and possibly about their subsequent behaviour32,38,39,49. The communicative function of yawning can have an adaptive role at inter-group level (i.e., defence from predators, vigilance50) because it promotes the synchronization of groups' activity through individuals that do not necessarily share strong bonds31,51&endash;54. Consistently, in wild lions Casetta et al.7 found that yawn contagion increased coordinated movements. Hence, also in wild geladas yawn contagion may be a phenomenon that promotes the coordination within larger ecological units, such as teams and, possibly, bands33.
 
The low effect size of the significant variables affecting the yawning response (Table _(Table1)1) cautions against the generalisation of the results and points toward the need of further data collection on other sites and populations. Despite this limitation, our findings open new scenarios on the role of yawn contagion in multilevel social systems. We suggest that this phenomenon operates at a multilayer scale reflecting the complex nested organization of geladas. At the basic level (core units), yawn contagion might improve synchronisation between group members sharing good relationship quality, as suggested for captive geladas35. In the wild, yawn contagion may also function at an upper level (team) by promoting the coordination of daily movements and activities between groups. Further studies may verify whether yawn contagion indeed elicits subsequent movements jointly started by individuals involved in the contagion not only between but also within groups (e.g.9).
 
On a broader perspective, our study underlines the importance of combining wild and captive approaches to thoroughly describe behavioural phenomena that are sensitive to different social and environmental conditions.