Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
http://www.baillement.com

mystery of yawning 

 

 

haut de page

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

mise à jour du
28 août 2015
Personnality and Indiidual Differences
2015;86:33-37
ContagiousYawning and Psychopathy
 
Brian K Rundle, Vanessa R Vaughn, Matthew S Stanford
 Baylor University, Department of Psychology and Neurooscience
 

Chat-logomini

 Tous les articles sur la contagion du bâillement
All articles about contagious yawning
 
Echokinetic yawning, theory of mind, and empathy
ABSTRACT
 
Psychopathy is characterized by a general antisocial lifestyle with behaviors including being selfish, manipulative, impulsive, fearless, callous, possibly domineering, and particularly lacking in empathy. Contagious yawning in our species has been strongly linked to empathy. We exposed 135 students, male and female, who completed the Psychopathic Personality Inventory-Revised (PPI-R), to a yawning paradigm intended to induce a reactionary yawn. Further, we exposed males to an emotion-related startle paradigm meant to assess peripheral amygdalar reactivity. We found that scores on the PPI-R subscale Coldheartedness significantly predicted a reduced chance of yawning. Further, we found that emotion-related startle amplitudes were predictive of frequency of contagious yawning. These data suggest that psychopathic traits may be related to the empathic nature of contagious yawning in our species.
 
1. Yawning and psychopathy
 
Yawning is a stereotyped behavior that, in our evolutionary history, has clear, deep roots as evidenced by its proliferation in mammals as well as many other vertebrates (Argiolas and Melis, 1998; Lehmann, 1979). It is clearly characterized by long inspiration followed by a shorter expiration (Argiolas and Melis, 1998). While literature concerning the pharmacology and functional anatomy of yawning is not lacking (Argiolas and Melis, 1998; Guggisberg, Mathis, Schnider, and Hess, 2010; Nahab, Hattori, Saad, and Hallett, 2009), the primary facet of yawning of interest is the phenomena of contagious yawns, specifically within the context of psychopathology.
 
Contagious yawns, which are spurred by yawn, thinking, hearing, reading, or observing another conspecific (or other species), have been linked to empathy (Lehmann, 1979; Platek, Critton, Myers, and Gallup, 2003; Platek, Mohamed, and Gallup, 2005). They are even documented in other familiar animals such as Pan Troglodytes and Canis Familiaris and have been linked to empathy (Campbell and de Waal, 2011; Romero, Konno, and Hasegawa, 2013). The anatomy and pharmacology of yawning and its contagious nature are beginning to be investigated, with oxytocin playing a large role as well as the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), precuneus, bilateral thalamus, and parahippocampal gyrus (PHG) (Platek et al., 2005; Sanna, Argiolas, and Melis, 2012). Interestingly, Schurmann et al. (2005) found that the mirror-neuron system is not directly activated in contagious yawning, suggesting that the action is automatic and not imitated. Norscia and Palagi (2011) found that people show a large susceptibility to contagious yawns when elicited by a related individual in terms of occurrence and frequency of yawns. For strangers, they found that people show a marked latency period of contagious yawns, strongly suggesting a component of familiarity involved with the contagion. Variations in susceptibility to contagious yawning are already known to occur in certain populations. Age is known to affect the likelihood of contagious yawning; as age increases, contagious yawning decreases (Bartholomew and Cirulli, 2014). Further, children on the autism spectrum are less likely to demonstrate contagious yawning (Giganti and Esposito Ziello, 2009; Senju et al., 2007), which is speculated to have a strong relationship to the empathetic deficits seen in this population.
 
1.1. Psychopathy
 
Empirical support for yawning having its evolutionary roots in empathic behavior is growing (Campbell and de Waal, 2011). Psychopathic traits, then, become a curious angle in which to view contagious yawning in our species. Psychopathy is characterized by a general antisocial lifestyle including being selfish, manipulative, impulsive, fearless, callous, domineering, and particularly lacking in empathy (Hare, 2003; Weber, Habel, Amunts, and Schneider, 2008). The disorder is typically assessed via the Psychopathic Check List-Revised (PCL-R) developed by Hare (2003) or the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI-R) developed by Lilienfeld and Widows (2005). Psychopathy and its close relative Antisocial Personality Disorder are found overwhelmingly in males (Cale and Lilienfeld, 2002). Additionally, psychopathy carries specific brain abnormalities including structural and functional impairments of the orbitofrontal-ventromedial prefrontal cortex as well as the amygdala (Gao, Glenn, Schug, Yang, and Raine, 2009; Weber et al., 2008).
 
The PPI-R operationalizes two discrete components within psychop-athy: a primary (affective) and secondary (behavioral) facet (Hare, 2003; Lilienfeld and Widows, 2005), where the primary facet encom-passes features including cruelty, lack of affect and empathy, while the secondary facet encompasses features such as impulsivity and aggres-sion. Psychopaths demonstrate an overall small but marked decrease in the ability to recognize emotion in others (Kosson, Suchy, Mayer, and Libby, 2002; Wilson, Juodis, and Porter, 2011), which is also associated with decreased amygdalar function, particularly with fearful faces (Jones, Laurens, Herba, Barker, and Viding, 2009). Kosson et al. (2002) showed a slight overall decreased ability to rec-ognize emotion, but a large deficit in recognizing disgust in others when the task involved non-verbal responses. It has also been shown that psychopaths fail to exhibit a conditioned response to aver-sive Pavlovian conditioning (Flor, Birbaumer, Hermann, Silvio, and Patrick, 2002), which suggests deficiencies in amygdala-dependent memory.
 
What sets psychopathy apart from its close relatives Conduct Disor-der and Antisocial Personality Disorder is its distinct emotional compo-nent. That is, psychopathy involves a prevalent emotional profile consisting of a considerable reduction in or lack of empathy (Frick, O'Brien, Wooton, and McBurnett, 1994; Hare, 2003). Psychopathy has also been found to be inversely related to the ability to perceive emotion (in both male and females) and managing emotion (only in men) (Lishner, Swim, Hong, and Vitacco, 2011).
 
12. Hypothesis
 
Given the nature of psychopathy and yawning discussed herein, the current study aims to examine the relationship between contagious yawning and psychopathic traits. This will be examined both by a yawn-ing paradigm designed by the current researchers (modeled after Platek et al., 2005) as well as an emotion-related startle paradigm (ERS) previ-ously used in Anderson, Stanford, Wan, and Young (2011). Affective po-tentiation of the acoustic startle reflex (by Electromyograph [EMG] and Galvanic skin response [GSR]) is one of the most prominent psycho-physiological measures of amygdalar responsiveness (Davis, 1989; Lang, Bradley, and Cuthbett, 1990; LeDoux, Iwata, Cicchetti, and Reis, 1988). Psychopaths reliably demonstrate an impairment of potentiation of the startle reflex (Patrick, Bradley, and Lang, 1993), while healthy controls reliably potentiate with negative affective valence and attenu-ate the fear response with positive affective valences (Lang et al., 1990). What's more, Patrick et al. (1993) ; Patrick (1994) connected the lack of potentiated startle in psychopathy to the emotional facet of the PCL-R (Hare, 2003) while the behavioral facet was found to be unre-lated. Further, given the growing evidence that contagious yawning and empathy are evolutionarily related, a connection between psychopathy and yawning maintains sufficient precedence. To our knowledge, such an examination has not been done in high psychopathic trait individuals, nor have contagious yawning been addressed using ERS. In our case, we expect to find a connection between psychopathic traits and a decreased susceptibility to conta-giously yawn.
 
4. Discussion
4.1. Experiment one
 
While psychopathy is not simply the lack of empathy, endorsement of the CD subscale is strongly indicative of damped empathic affect. The theory that contagious yawning in our species is largely mediated by empathy is supported by the significant difference between genders on probability of yawning (63.5% of women yawned compared to 53.1% of males), as females tend to score higher on measures of em-pathy than males (Rueckett, Branch, and Doan, 2011). Further, as previ-ously discussed, psychopathy is almost exclusively diagnosed in males.
 
It is expected that a higher percentage of females would yawn dur-ing the paradigm; however, since there was no statistical difference between genders, a gender effect on likelihood to yawn can be easily controlled. This difference merely shows that the paradigm itself maintains a level of validity consistent with the existing literature that would be expected. It is true that this is an indirect expectation by means of empathy; to our knowledge, no conclusive evidence between gender and contagious yawning has been established (Baenninger, Binkley, and Baenninger, 1996).
 
When evaluating the all subscales, only CD yielded significant re-sults. This is not surprising considering that the grouping variables SCI or FD do not wholly capture the emotional component (or lack thereof) of psychopathy and tend to focus on behavioral and interpersonal factors. A difference between groups suggests that increased CD is associated with decreased susceptibility to a conta-gious yawn. Further, when considering the overall PPI-R score, which includes all subscales, 1 a strong trend towards significance is seen, which is likely mediated by Coldheartedness. When compared to other sub-factors, FD and SCI, Coldheartedness clearly maintains the largest affective component on the PPI-R (Lilienfeld and Widows, 2005).
 
In line with theory and previously shown between genders, in-creased empathy is associated with increased susceptibility to conta-gious yawning (Platek et al., 2003). Using psychopathy as a predictor of susceptibility of contagious yawning may, then, be a viable avenue of research simply by virtue of the interpersonal and emotional abnor-malities associated with psychopathy. The results of this experiment lend support to the theory that empathy and contagious yawning are related in our species and may work together to accomplish social ends.
 
4.2. Experiment two
 
Though only males were exposed to the startle paradigm as part of a separate study, yawning data were collected on all participants. A re-gression analysis showed that the difference between the negative and neutral mean potentiation was predictive of yawning frequency. Experiment 2 provides physiological data and a predictive parametric measure for future study. In this case, the lower the yawning suscepti-bility, the lower the difference between neutral and negative startle re-sponse (resulting in an overall higher numerical value). The negative correlation between the neutral-negative difference and yawning fre-quency shows that the higher the frequency of yawns, the greater the difference between the neutral-negative difference (resulting in a more negative numerical value). Thus, the greater the measured psy-chopathic traits, the lower the startle potentiation and, therefore, the lower difference value between neutral and negative. On the other hand, individuals with lower levels of psychopathic traits showed greater startle potentiation, which produces a greater difference value.
 
The relationship between startle potentiation and emotional dysreg-ulation is well studied. Aside from the psychopathy-related research currently presented, startle potentiation is seen in emotional disorders involving increased (and dysregulated) emotional disposition including anxiety and depression (see Ballard et al., 2014). The available research gives sufficient pretense to the data collected in these experiments. Our data suggest that one's level of psychopathic traits and startle reactivity are related to one's susceptibility to contagious yawning. That is to say, a well-established physiological measure of psychopathic traits is also highly associated with the susceptibility to contagious yawning. The less emotion-related startle reactivity observed, the lower the susceptibility to contagiously yawn. Both independent variables have a strong relationship to empathic behavior and emotional regulation, and taken together, provide insight into the complex nature of yawning and social interaction. While no conclusion can be drawn at this time, these data are steps forward in elucidating the function of contagious yawning in our species. Further, this invites inquiry into amygdalar reactivity as measured by ERS and the PCC, precuneus, thalamus, and PHG, involved in empathy and self-referent processing (Platek et al., 2005). Platek et al. suggests these areas may be related to the general sensory aspects of face-processing.
 
5. Conclusion
 
To our knowledge, no experiments showing the relationship between startle potentiation, psychopathy, and yawning have been published. It is, however, established the relationship between psychopathy and startle as well as the relationship between yawning and empathy (Norscia and Palagi, 2011). Though pilot in nature, our data suggests that startle potentiation to negative stimuli may predict one's susceptibility to contagious yawning. In line with the theories presented on yawning and startle, it is reasonable to expect that low startle potentiation is related to yawning susceptibility, as affect is highly considered in both realms of research. There is also an evolutionary justification for the results found here-in. While psychopathic traits are not the direct inverse of empathetic traits, both are constructs that seem to capture polarized behaviors.
 
Coldheartedness refers to a dearth of social emotion (Lilienfeld and Widows, 2005). That is, it is an inconsideration of the emotional state of others. Our results, then, fit well into the evolutionary model that contagious yawning in our species is a function of empathy, as we have shown that those who are characteristically lacking in empathy are less susceptible to a contagious yawn when prompted in a paradigm known to induce contagious yawning in normal individuals (Platek et al., 2003).
 
The results of these two experiments are clear indicators that psychopathy is a robust, multifaceted disposition, where a strict interpretation of an overall PPI-R score is not necessarily a predictive one. Rather, attention to subscales and, of course, clinical evaluations are clearly more appropriate for predictability. While gender effects between yawning susceptibility may exist, the gender effect is controlled in the current study. Of course, the use of females in the startle-yawn paradigm would be important in future studies, as psychopathy is not a sex-dependent phenomenon (Anderson et al., 2011). The emotional component of the PPI-R is likely the most relevant to the experiments herein. While the overall measure is possibly too broad a measure for these purposes, it nonetheless lends support to the developing idea that psychopathy, empathy, and contagious yawning are related.