Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
http://www.baillement.com

mystery of yawning 

 

mise à jour du
10 mars 2020
 
Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
2020;111:149-165.
The neuroethology of spontaneous mimicry
and emotional contagion
in human and non-human animals  
 
Palagi E, Celeghin A, Tamietto M, Winkielman P, Norscia I.
 

Chat-logomini

 Tous les articles sur la contagion du bâillement
All articles about contagious yawning
 
Abstract
Spontaneous mimicry appears fundamental to emotional perception and contagion, especially when it involves facial emotional expressions. Here we cover recent evidence on spontaneous mimicry from ethology, psychology and neuroscience, in non-human and human animals. We first consider how mimicry unfolds in non-human animals (particularly primates) and how it relates to emotional contagion. We focus on two forms of mimicry-related phenomena: facial mimicry and yawn contagion, which are largely conserved across mammals and useful to draw evolutionary scenarios. Next, we expand on the psychological evidence from humans that bears on current theoretical debates and also informs non-human animal research. Finally, we cover the neural bases of facial mimicry and yawn contagion. We move beyond the perception/expression/experience trichotomy and from the correlational to the causal evidence that links facial mimicry to emotional contagion by presenting evidence from neuroimaging, direct manipulation, neuro-stimulation and lesion studies. In conclusion, this review proposes a bottom-up, multidisciplinary approach to the study of spontaneous mimicry that accounts for the evolutionary continuity linking non-human and human animals.
 
Yawn contagion
 
Even though spontaneous yawning is considered a Þxed action pattern, its description is not Þxed at all. Spontaneous yawning has been described as consisting of long, deep inspiration, brief peak with apnea (acme) followed by a slow expiration (Walusinski and Deputte, 2004; Guggisberg et al., 2010) sometimes reported as rapid (Baenninger, 1997; Krestel et al., 2018) or shorter (Provine, 2012; Gallup et al., 2016). When integrating the di_erent descriptions of the motor patterns deÞning a yawning event (Walusinski and Deputte, 2004; Guggisberg et al., 2010; Provine, 2012; Baenninger, 1997), it is possible to claim that it includes active jaw gaping, eye closure, con- traction of facial muscles, and passive jaw closure, accompanied by neck stretching and head tilting and, in some cases, by limb and body stretching. Due to its complicated combination of patterns, Provine (2012) distinguished di_erent types of yawn (close-nose yawn; clen- ched-teeth yawn; sealed-lips nose yawn; the eyes-open yawns) showing that some features of yawning can be modulated (Krestel et al., 2017). For all these reasons, a yawning pattern includes a facial expression but involves more than just facial muscles.
 
Once elicited, yawning cannot be completely suppressed. Therefore, it has been also deÞned as a stereotyped or reþex-like pattern (Lehmann, 1979; Provine and Hamernik, 1986). Barbizet (1958, p.203) deÞned yawning 'halfway between a reþex and an expressive move- ment'. Indeed, the morphological variability surrounding the yawning display indicates that this behavioural phenomenon is more articulated than a simple reþex (Massen and Gallup, 2017). However, despite the di_erent morphological variants found in some primate species (e.g. chimpanzees; Vick and Paukner, 2010; geladas: Palagi et al., 2009; macaques: Zannella et al., 2017; humans:Provine and Hamernik, 1986, 2012), the basic yawning pattern is plesiomorphic and well recogniz- able across species (Baenninger, 1987). This makes yawning an ex- cellent behavioural marker to investigate facial display replication in a comparative perspective.
 
As a physiological response, yawning can be inþuenced by internal and external factors (e.g. time of the day: Giganti and Zilli, 2011; in- tracranial/brain temperature; Gallup and Eldakar, 2013). Yawning is also socially modulated: it is more likely to occur in real social settings, as a result of yawn contagion (Provine, 1989, 2005). However, in Vir- tual Reality (VR) trials, the physical presence of a researcher during testing signiÞcantly inhibited contagious yawning, even though parti- cipants were viewing a virtual environment (and virtual yawns) and were unable to see the researcher (Gallup et al., 2019).
 
The same factors that a_ect spontaneous yawning are observed in yawn contagion. Contagious yawning, although socially modulated, remains a physiological response. It has been found in several animal species, including humans, and it occurs when individuals respond with a yawn after perceiving a yawning from/in others (Provine, 2005; Norscia and Palagi, 2011; Palagi et al., 2009; Demuru and Palagi, 2012; Campbell and de Waal, 2011, 2014; Romero et al., 2013).
 
The phenomenon of yawn contagion seems to rely upon the per- ception-action mechanism also involving the mirror neuron system (Preston and de Waal, 2002; Gallese et al., 2004). Kapitány and Nielsen (2017) suggested that in Homo sapiens the increased occurrence of yawning in social conditions - as compared to solitary - may be due not to contagion but to the social setting itself. However, their study showed that the mere presence of others was not su_cient to increase the probability of yawning. Instead, in line with what is expected when contagion is in place, they found that the yawning rates were sig- niÞcantly higher in the social non-blind (individuals could see each other and detect others' yawns) than in the blind condition (individuals could not see each other). In both conditions, the auditory component of yawns was excluded by having the participants listening to Chopin's Complete Nocturnes (Kapitany and Nielsen, 2017).
 
Yawn contagion has been found in all the hominine species: chim- panzees (Pan troglodytes: Anderson et al., 2004; Campbell and de Waal, 2011; Campbell and Cox, 2019), bonobos (Pan paniscus: Demuru and Palagi, 2012; Tan et al., 2017; but see: Amici et al., 2014 on a vary small sample size) and humans (Homo sapiens: Provine and Hamernik, 1986; 1989). Outside the hominine species but still within the hominid family, two reports failed to Þnd yawn contagion in lowland gorillas
 
(Gorilla gorilla; Amici et al., 2014; on a larger sample including a ba- chelor and a harem group: Palagi et al., 2019a, 2019b). On the other hand, yawn contagion has been found in two cercopithecid monkey species (geladas, Theropithecus gelada: Palagi et al., 2009; Tonkean macaque, Macaca tonkeana: Palagi and Norscia, 2019) but not convin- cingly in others. SpeciÞcally, yawn contagion has not been found in the Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata, Palagi and Norscia, 2019) and the situation is unclear for stump-tailed macaques (Macaca arctoides; Paukner and Anderson, 2005). Stump-tailed macaques yawned sig- niÞcantly more while watching a yawn video than a control video but also showed more self-directed behaviour like scratching, which in primates is related to anxiety (Paukner and Anderson, 2005). Results from stump-tailed macaques suggests that the yawning response may be also linked to anxiety and not necessarily to contagion. Beyond pri- mates, yawn contagion has been found in wolves (Canis lupus lupus; Romero et al., 2014) and domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris; inter- speciÞc yawn contagion; Silva et al., 2012; Romero et al., 2013). In dogs the yawning response was found not associated with stress but only with the perception of a triggering yawn emitted by the owner (Romero et al., 2013). Therefore, it seems that phylogenetic closeness is not, per se, predictive of yawn contagion.