mystery of yawning
Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
http://www.baillement.com
resolutionmini

mise à jour du
24 octobre 2010
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
2011;35(3):765-9
Why do we yawn? Primitive versus derived features
 
Gallup AC.
 
Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Princeton University, USA

Chat-logomini

Andrew C. Gallup. Yawning and the thermoregulatory hypothesis
 
Abstract
 
Guggisberg et al. (2010) reviewed the evidence for the origin and function of yawning, and conclude that theories describing a physiological role lack support. Instead, they argue research supports the notion that yawning has a communicative function. Contrary to the authors' claim that the social/communication hypothesis has the "best experimental evidence", there is in fact no definitive experimental support for the predictions of this model. Furthermore, the authors claim to take an evolutionary perspective, but sufficient examples across the comparative (non-primate) literature are missing, and they fail to acknowledge phylogenic history. Due to the ubiquity of this behavior across vertebrates, and the regularity of its occurrence in a number of different physiological states and social contexts, it is likely that instead of serving one purpose, yawning is multifunctional across a number of species. The most parsimonious explanation for the origin of yawning suggests that any social value is a derived feature, while the primitive feature or function is physiological. The current paper addresses these concerns, and identifies a number of other weaknesses in the social/communication hypothesis as a global explanation for the origin and function of yawning.
 
1. Introduction
 
This paper discusses the following shortcomings associated with the review on yawning by Guggisberg et al. (2010): the lack of a comparative analysis, the inability to incorporate instances of excessive or atypical yawning, an inadequate review of the existing physiological hypotheses, problems associated with the description and predictions of the social / communication hypothesis, the mismatch of standards and criteria used to evaluate the various hypotheses, the lack of experimental support for the social/communication hypothesis, problems associated with this hypothesis as an explanation of the phylogenic origin and function of yawning, and the lack of a distinction between primitive and derived features. One major problem with the Guggisberg et al. (2010) review is their stance on the origin of yawning as social. Any social or communicative value of yawning among humans and nonhuman primates is likely a derived feature, while the underlying primitive feature or function is physiological. Due to the ubiquity of this behavior across vertebrates, and the regularity of its occurrence in a number of different physiological states and social contexts, it is probably the case that instead of serving one purpose, yawning is multifunctional across a number of species.
 
2. Comparative aspects of yawning
 
Guggisberg et al. (2010) begin their review on the origin and functional hypotheses of yawning by discussing the ubiquity of yawning, or yawn-like behaviors across vertebrate classes. Classifying yawning versus open-mouth gestures or jaw stretching is often difficult, and no one has systematically defined a complete action repertoire of yawning that would accommodate all observed cases. Comparatively, it is customary to assume that yawning consists of an extended gaping of the mouth, followed by a more rapid closure (Baenninger, 1987; Provine, 1986). Among birds and mammals, however, yawning is accompanied by a deep inhalation of air with eye closure, followed by a shorter expiration. Although fish yawns have been described, they are fundamentally different from those of terrestrial vertebrates, and have been characterized to be separate from yawning in birds and mammals (Rasa, 1971). Other reports on Siamese fighting fish (Betta spiendens) support this view, reflecting aggressive social displays more than spontaneous yawning events (Baenninger, 1987). So-called yawning in snakes may also be different from other accounts, as it is not a spontaneous action, but a motor pattern following feeding strikes and swallowing that functions in the stretching and realignment of the jaw (Dullemeijer and Povel, 1972). Baenninger (1987) argues that although amphibians and reptiles open their mouth widely on occasion, this does not necessarily represent yawning. Guggisberg et al. (2010) acknowledge this controversy surrounding the comparative aspects of yawning initially, but then fail to further describe the phylogeny of yawning in relation to various hypotheses throughout the review.
 
3. Atypical yawning and medical conditions
 
Considering the close association between atypical yawning and a number of diseases or medical conditions in humans (Daquin et al., 2001; Gallup and Gallup, 2008; Walusinski, 2009), it is clear that the study of yawning and its functions has important implications for medical research and diagnoses. Likewise, only after we have achieved a thorough understanding of the pharmacology and physiology of yawning can interpretations be made regarding functionality. One underlying problem with the review by Guggisberg et al. (2010) is that they sidestep this literature to focus on normal yawning. Understanding the pathology of a behavior in its association with medical conditions or drug-use can provide intriguing insights regarding its triggers and functions. If the authors want to evaluate the various functional hypotheses in an attempt to account for all instances of yawning, then it is imperative that excessive and pathological yawning be discussed in detail. The authors briefly mention important work on the neuropharmacology of yawning (e.g., Argiolas and Melis, 1998), but fail to integrate it into the later sections of their review.
 
4. Review of physiological hypotheses
 
Another shortcoming of the Guggisberg et al. (2010) paper is that it does not sufficiently review the literature on existing physiological theories. For instance, the thermoregulatory hypothesis has far more support than they acknowledge (reviewed by Gallup et al., 2010). In addition, recent research has explored the relationship between brain temperature and yawning by implanting thermocoupled probes in the prelimbic cortex of rats (Rattus norvegicus) to measure changes in brain temperature before, during and after yawning (Shoup-Knox et al., 2010). Results show that yawning was preceded in all instances by rapid increases in brain temperature (0.12°C), with corresponding decreases in temperature occurring immediately after each yawn (Shoup-Knox et al., 2010). Recent comparative research is consistent with this, showing a strong negative correlation between body temperatures and yawn latency in handling-stressed budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) (Miller et al., 2010). Although a detailed review of the association between yawning and thermoregulation (Gallup and Gallup, 2008) is mentioned in passing, it is otherwise largely overlooked.
 
Guggisberg et al. (2010) also inadequately describe how yawning could provide cerebral cooling, not taking into account other effects described by this model (i.e., increased circulation). In addition, Guggisberg et al. (2010) attempt to discredit the results of Gallup and Gallup (2007), which showed that methods of behavioral cooling (i.e., nasal breathing and forehead cooling) diminish yawning, by arguing that it is impossible to differentiate the effects of temperature and sleepiness. However, this argument does not take into account the fact that nasal breathing (in the absence of mouth closure) reduces yawning while breathing orally leaves yawning rates unaffected (Study 1, Gallup and Gallup, 2007). Nasal breathing, for instance, does not inhibit sleepiness, and therefore the temperature/sleep confound described by Guggisberg et al. (2010) is untenable. According to the reasoning presented by Guggisberg et al. (2010), the association between warm temperature and sleep would predict that yawning should increase when ambient temperatures are held high. Contrary to these predictions, experimental research on budgerigars shows that yawning is reduced at constant high temperatures, and this is not an artifact of increased sleep or rest (Gallup et al., 2009). Therefore, despite the view of Guggisberg et al. (2010), this research directly distinguishes between temperature and sleep effects, and these results are in accord with the predictions of the thermal window hypothesis of the thermoregulatory model.
 
The authors also fail to provide a detailed description of the vast comparative evidence in support of the arousal hypothesis reviewed originally by Baenninger (1997). To date, this review provides the most comprehensive account of the comparative research on yawning. Instead of describing the support for this hypothesis, the majority of the section on arousal is used to argue against it (Guggisberg et al., 2010). Central to this argument is the discussion of an earlier study by members of the same laboratory investigating changes in electroencephalogram (EEG) associated with yawning (Guggisberg et al., 2007), but the authors fail to describe the shortcomings of their report. For instance, the participants used in this study were patients who suffered from excessive daytime sleepiness or non-restorative sleep (Guggisberg et al., 2007). Considering the close connection between yawning and sleep, it is possible that the results are unique to this subset of individuals, and that similar results would not be found in a normal, and healthy population. In addition, these changes in EEG were only measured during a 10-s window (Guggisberg et al., 2007). Therefore, it is entirely possible that yawning may increase arousal over a longer time, and in ways that are not detected by EEG. An extension and replication of this study is needed.
 
5. Problems with the social/communication hypothesis
 
As stated, the social/communication hypothesis is extremely vague. According to this hypothesis, yawning is a form of nonverbal communication that signals one's current mental- or physiological-state. However, descriptions of the states being communicated are highly ambiguous. Phrases such as "certain body states", "mildly to moderately unpleasant", and "unpleasant but not immediately threatening states" are vague and imprecise, and the use of these descriptions demonstrates the scarce research investigating predictions of the social/communication hypothesis. For instance, Guggisberg et al. (2010) speculate that the reason yawning is suppressed among humans when being observed (Baenninger and Greco, 1991; Provine, 2005) is because individuals find the situation "inappropriate", and that they "hide" their yawns. But the authors neither provide any solid evidence for this assertion, nor do they go on to describe what distinguishes yawns that are felt inappropriate from those that are not. There is simply not enough scientific support for the social/communication hypothesis, and attempts to construct post hoc arguments in its favor can easily be dismissed.
 
Earlier in the review the authors claimed that if 'the act of yawning does not induce more autonomic changes than the ones that already occur hundreds of times throughout the day due to simple breathing or moving" then it does not make sense to propose a circulatory function to yawning when more efficient or effective behaviors could be employed (e.g., other body movements). However, the authors fail to acknowledge that this same argument can apply to the signaling value of yawning according to the social/communication hypothesis. For instance, signals must be detected and distinguished from background noise, and it is not hard to see how a more transparent signal would achieve social/communicative functions with much less interference. On the other hand, if yawning does produce a meaningful signal that is easily interpreted by others (for which there is no available evidence), then the efficiency or strength of the signal is not important. Similarly, since yawning produces significant changes in circulation (increased heart rate, vasodilation, and cerebral blood flow) (Greco and Baenninger, 1991; Guggisberg et al., 2007; Schroth and Klose, 1992) then it too produces a physiological outcome. Many of the arguments made by Guggisberg et al. (2010) in an attempt to undermine the physiological hypotheses can also be used to raise serious questions about the social/communication hypothesis.
 
Problems continue to arise with the logic used for the argument that the origin and function of yawning is social or communicative. In the most primitive sense, a signal is a behavior or structure that has evolved to alter the behavior of another organism (Maynard Smith and Harper, 2003). Thus, signals need to be interpreted and recognizable by others, and mechanisms need to be in place to receive these signals. Comparative yawning research in support of these predictions is lacking. There is no comparative evidence that group members orient towards or receive this "signal." Similarly, research on the response of others (i.e., altered behavior) to yawning is absent. Although threat yawns have been described in some primates (Hinde and Tinbergen, 1958; Redican, 1975; Tinbergen, 1952), the notion that canine displays during yawns are aggressive signals is a highly controversial topic (Deputte, 1994; Dobson, 2010). Furthermore, "threat yawns" are fundamentally different from spontaneous yawns in that they are assumed to only occur in specific social contexts. Therefore, there is reason to believe such yawns have different physiological consequences and triggers than spontaneous yawns, and they may not be useful for the purposes of identifying the origin of this behavior.
 
The social/communication hypothesis falls short in a number of other important and basic areas. For one, signal evolution predicts that a signal will only evolve if the sender transmits information that is reliable to the receiver. Since yawning occurs with regularity under a variety of states such as fatigue, boredom, and stress, any signal that is displayed remains nonspecific. That is, unless the information transferred from yawning is context-dependent, meaning that receivers can reliably discern the signal of yawns from individuals in different environments. There is no research exploring this question, and in fact results from of an in-depth study of yawning in Macaca fascicularis and Lophocebus albigena suggest that aside from during resting, yawning is not associated with any particular social context (Deputte, 1994). Therefore, it is unlikely that yawning outside of resting could reliably transmit relevant information regarding a particular situation. The authors
 
fail to sufficiently address this seemingly obvious issue. Instead, they state that all triggers for yawning "have in common that they are mildly to moderately unpleasant while not representing an immediate threat." In addition, in order for a signal to remain reliable, the receiver must benefit from having information about that attribute (Searcy and Nowicki, 2005). There is no comparative evidence suggesting that individuals who simply witness others yawning receive any benefit. On the other hand, if the physiological consequences of yawning provide some homeostatic function (i.e., arousal, thermoregulation), then yawning in response to perceiving another yawn (contagious yawning) may provide a benefit to the receiver (Gallup and Gallup, 2007). But of course this would only hold true for species that have a capacity for contagious yawning (an issue discussed below). Secondly, in the description of yawning as a social signal, Guggisberg et al. (2010) do not mention honest versus dishonest signaling, or the prospect of any cost associated with yawning. Well-established theories regarding signal evolution have posited that a signal is likely to entail some cost to the sender if it is to remain honest (Zahavi, 1975; Grafen, 1990; Andersson, 1994). For instance, vulnerability or production costs may be associated with yawning during certain circumstances, as a yawn may draw unwanted attention to the sender, momentarily decrease vigilance, or conflict with antipredatory behaviors (Miller et al., 2010). Although alternative explanations exist for understanding the evolution of signal reliability (Searcy and Nowicki, 2005), the fact that the authors do not discuss this issue, but proclaim yawning as primarily social, is yet another shortcoming.
 
In addition, the social/communication hypothesis is not consistent with some of the empirical research on yawning and stress. Guggisberg et al. (2010) argue that yawns are triggered by a stressor to communicate an individual's mental- or physiological-state, but recent research on budgerigars has shown that yawning is initially suppressed following the application of stressors (Miller et al., 2010). This response more accurately describes a recovery behavior following the stressor, and not a communicative signal of the stressor itself.
 
6. Support for the social/communication hypothesis
 
The majority of the evidence in support for the social/communication hypothesis hinges on the contagious nature of this behavior in humans (Provine, 1986, l989a,b; Platek et al., 2003) and a select few non-human primates (Anderson et al., 2004; Paukner and Anderson, 2006; Palagi et al., 2009), yet this is considered "good" evidence for the origin and function of yawning to be communication. Contagious yawning has also been reported in dogs (Canis familiaris) in response to human yawns (Joy-Mascheroni et al., 2008), but a further study shows that dogs do not yawn contagiously to conspecifics (Harr et al., 2009). The fact that yawning is triggered during drowsiness, boredom and stress is also considered "good" support for the social/communication hypothesis (Guggisberg et al., 2010). It is further stated that the social/communication hypothesis is the most parsimonious of all models, but this is not the only model that can account for social effects of yawning such as its contagiousness or the different physiological states and social contexts that can trigger it. In fact, arousal, state change, and thermoregulatory models do just as well, and it is easy to argue that physiological models are more parsimonious. For instance, considering that yawns are triggered during low states of vigilance, and produce changes in underlying physiology (e.g., increased facial and cerebral blood flow), the spreading of this behavior to nearby conspecifics could enhance group vigilance (Gallup and Gallup, 2007). This interpretation actually provides more explanatory value than the former in that it is sequential in explaining the initial trigger(s) for yawning, and then accounts for the infectious nature of this behavior among a few species.
 
As if the evidence for the social/communication hypothesis was self-evident, Guggisberg et al. (2010) state that yawning may be used to communicate states to group-members "in order to enhance behavioral synchronization." These statements are not based on scientific evidence. Furthermore, they continue by stating that the social/communication hypothesis has the "best experimental evidence" among all existing hypotheses. But the fact of the matter is that the social/communication hypothesis has not been experimentally tested.Just as the predictions for physiological models indicate that yawning acts to alter "up- or downturns of a given body state", the main prediction of the social/communication hypothesis states that yawning synchronizes group behavior. Other than the act of yawning itself being replicated, studies of contagious yawning do not provide any evidence of the sort that group behavior has been synchronized. Observational studies of group resting or sleeping (e.g., Deputte, 1994) cannot be considered synchrony from yawning without proper controls for circadian effects (e.g., body temperature, general physiology, etc.).
 
The social/communication hypothesis is not supported by the largest body of experimental evidence among all propositions for explaining the origin and function of yawning, and in fact, the predictions of this hypothesis have never been experimentally tested. The authors even contradict their position by stating, "Missing elements of this model include controlled studies observing the regulating effect of yawning on synchronized group behavior" (the main and foremost prediction of this hypothesis). According to Guggisberg et al. (2010), the physiological models fail because there is insufficient support for the predictions of each, yet the authors do not use the same criteria when thinking about the social/communication hypothesis. Although the authors argue otherwise, the fact that yawning is commonly thought of as a sign of boredom or disrespect in many cultures (Schiller, 2002) can be taken as evidence against the social/communication hypothesis. This seemingly universal perception exemplifies the lack of informative communicative value to yawning, and how unconscious and uncontrolled physiological responses such as yawning can be misinterpreted in various social contexts. Yawning may have a social role in some primates (humans included), but to this point any function remains largely unclear.
 
7. Primitive versus derived features: an evolutionary perspective on yawning
 
Supporting the view that yawning is phylogenetically old, the onset of this behavior occurs quite early in uterine development (11 weeks gestation in humans) (de Vries et al., 1982). When thinking about yawning, or any evolutionarily conserved behavior, it is important to consider primitive versus derived features. Primitive features of a trait can be traced to the last common ancestor, while derived features are present only in more recently evolved organisms. Guggisberg et al. (2010) state how the social/communication hypothesis stems from the view that the trigger for yawning is physiological. Therefore, the underlying primitive feature of yawning is physiological, and any subsequent social or communicative role is a derived feature. The divergence of contagious yawns during more recent evolutionary history is strong evidence in support of it being a derived feature. On the other hand, the ubiquitous nature of spontaneous and isolated yawns represents the primitive, or underlying feature associated with this behavior. It is simply inaccurate to describe the origin of yawning to be social or communicative. As discussed earlier, behavioral synchronization has not been experimentally studied, but if it does exist, it is a derived feature of yawning. Given the objective of Guggisberg et al. (2010) to review the available literature on the origin and function of yawning, it is troubling that these distinctions were not discussed.
 
The authors argue, from an evolutionary perspective, that the social/communication hypothesis is sufficient to explain selection for yawning among all vertebrate species (Guggisberg et al., 2010). However, they do not take into account that the first jawed vertebrates were fishes, and presumably the first to yawn (Baenninger, 1987), yet these animals lack yawning contagion and any social role of yawning described by social/communication hypothesis. Furthermore, the authors state "There is therefore no need to postulate additional physiological functions of yawning to explain its selection during evolution." This assertion has many weaknesses. First, if yawning originally evolved for communicative purposes, it would imply that the common ancestor, as well as any subsequent descendants, should be particularly sensitive to perceived yawns. There is no support for this. Second, the communication hypothesis pertains solely to group-oriented species, yet the authors do not discuss the persistence of yawning among solitary vertebrates, or why some of the most social/gregarious mammals on the planet (ungulates) have been described to rarely, if ever yawn (Barbizet, 1958; Craemer, 1924; Heusner, 1946). Statements such as 'We might have to get used to the idea that yawns have a primarily social rather than physiological function" are highly problematic and misleading. As noted previously, experimentally supported social aspects of yawning (i.e., contagion) are limited to a few primate species, while other primary social/communicative roles of yawning (i.e., behavioral synchronization) have yet to be established.
 
8. Conclusions
 
Given the ubiquitous nature of yawning, it is likely multifunctional across species. And when considering the potential multifunctionality of this behavior, as well as the presence of derived features in certain lineages, it should be studied on a species by species basis. Any differences in form and function should be anchored to the underlying physiology and the unique evolutionary histories and ecological adaptations of that species. As previously incorporated into the thermoregulatory model (Gallup et al., 2010), one potentially fruitful distinction for making these predictions may come through the classification of homeothermic (birds, mammals) versus poikilothermic (fish, amphibians, reptiles) vertebrates. No one theory is likely to explain all manifestations or derived functions of yawning, and since experimental evidence of contagious yawning is only observed in a small number of species in one lineage (i.e., primates), the notion that the origin of yawning is social/communicative can be easily dismissed. It is inaccurate to conclude that the social/communication hypothesis has the best experimental evidence when in fact the main predictions have not been tested. Statements regarding any social function of yawning should be tempered until much more comparative research is conducted. The disproportionate focus of Guggisberg et al. (2010) on human-related studies may confound and distort the basic nature of this ubiquitous behavior. The stated "simplicity and elegance" of the social/communication model needs to stand up to rigorous experimental hypothesis testing before it can be accepted as a viable account of yawning.
 
 

Andrew C. Gallup. Yawning and the thermoregulatory hypothesis