|
- mise à jour
du
- 18 août
2025
- Curr
Zool
- 2024;71(2):137-151
|
- Yawning
in sync:
- implications
for social cohesion in horses
- Alice Galotti, Martina Romano,
- Paolo Baragli, Elisabetta Palagi
|

|
- Abstract
- The increasing interest in the study of
spontaneous (SY) and contagious yawning (CY) was
so far focused on several taxa, especially
primates. Here, we focused on SY and CY in
horses, a suitable species due to their complex
social dynamics that has been largely overlooked
in research on these phenomena. By analyzing
videos of 48 horses on pasture, we identified 2
yawning morphologies: Covered (Y __) and
Uncovered Teeth (Y UcT). Using EquiFACS, we
quantitatively demonstrated that Y __ and Y uct
differ in terms of muscle recruitment. Moreover,
we provide the first evidence for the presence
of CY by comparing 2 different conditions:
chewing-yawn-chewing versus
chewingchewing-chewing. Supporting the Social
Modulation hypothesis, in our mares, CY was more
prominent among subjects sharing good
relationships. Moreover, subjects responded more
rapidly to kin compared with non-kin and kin
frequently grooming each other responded even
more rapidly to each other yawns. The high
familiar yawn sensitivity can provide selective
advantages increasing behavioral synchronization
and group cohesion.
-
- Résumé
- L'intérêt croissant pour
l'étude des bâillements
spontanés (SY) et contagieux (CY) s'est
jusqu'à présent concentré
sur plusieurs taxons, en particulier les
primates. Ici, les auteurs se sont
concentrés sur le SY et le CY chez les
chevaux, une espèce appropriée en
raison de sa dynamique sociale complexe qui a
été largement
négligée dans la recherche sur ces
phénomènes. En analysant les
vidéos de 48 chevaux au pâturage,
nous avons identifié 2 morphologies de
bâillements : Les dents couvertes (Y __)
et les dents non couvertes (Y UcT). En utilisant
EquiFACS, nous avons démontré
quantitativement que Y __ et Y uct
diffèrent en termes de recrutement
musculaire. De plus, ils fournissent la
première preuve de la présence de
CY en comparant 2 conditions différentes
: mâcher-yawn-mâcher versus
mâcher-mâcher-mâcher. A
l'appui de l'hypothèse de la modulation
sociale, chez ces juments, le CY était
plus important chez les sujets partageant de
bonnes relations. En outre, les sujets
réagissaient plus rapidement aux proches
qu'aux non-apparentés et les proches qui
se toilettent fréquemment
répondaient encore plus rapidement aux
bâillements de l'autre. La forte
sensibilité aux bâillements
familiers peut fournir des avantages
sélectifs augmentant la synchronisation
comportementale et la cohésion du
groupe.
-

-
- Yawning is present in all vertebrates, from
the fetal stage to the adult age (Blanton 1917;
Schiller 2002; Provine 2005; Matikainen and Elo
2008; Massen et al. 2021; Gallup 2022). It is
considered a fixed action pattern being
unstoppable, uncontrollable, and morphologically
similar across species (Deputte 1994; Walusinski
and Deputte 2004; Provine 2010). From a
mechanistic perspective, yawning involves
basically 3 different phases: 1) a slow and wide
opening of the mouth, with a deep inhalation, 2)
a quick closure of the mouth accompanied, and 3)
by a brief exhalation (Barbizet 1958).
-
- Although challenging to distinguish, the
functions attributed to yawning can be
categorized based on the distinct physiological
conditions of the yawner (Physiological domain)
and the social situations in which yawning
occurs (Social domain) (Guggisberg et al. 2010).
Obviously, all these hypotheses, both between
and within domains, are not necessarily mutually
exclusive, probably being the 2 faces of the
same coin (e.g., proximate vs ultimate
explanations). Yawning can be implicated in
regulating physiological functions, including
blood oxygen levels (Guggisberg et al. 2010),
thermoregulation (Miller et al. 2010; Gallup and
Eldakar 2013), brain cooling (Gallup and Eldakar
2013), states of drowsiness and arousal (Deputte
1994; Zilli et al. 2008; Guggisberg et al. 2010;
Gallup 2022). The Thermoregulation hypothesis
suggests that yawning helps reduce body and
brain temperature, responding to environmental
temperature changes (Massen et al. 2014; Eldakar
et al. 2015; Massen and Gallup 2016). The
Drowsiness hypothesis links yawning to changes
in alertness during periods of inactivity, and
it is largely explored in both humans (Greco et
al. 1993; Provine 2005; Giganti et al. 2010;
Gallup and Meyers, 2021) and in different
non-human mammals (Otaria flavescens, Palagi et
al. 2019; Loxodonta africana, Rossman et al.
2017; Crocuta crocuta, Casetta et al. 2022; Pan
troglodytes, Vick and Paukner 2010; and
Theropithecus gelada,Leone et al. 2014). The
Brain Cooling Hypothesis, a form of
thermoregulation, suggests that yawning
dissipates heat by increasing blood flow, acting
as a radiator (Gallup and Gallup 2008; Gallup et
al. 2016, 2020; Massen et al. 2021). Lowering
brain temperature improves mental performance
and alertness. Yawning promotes cooling through
inhaling cool air (Gallup and Gallup 2008;
Gallup et al. 2016; Massen et al. 2021).
Baenninger (1997) highlights that spontaneous
yawning can be also influenced by social
context. The Social Distress hypothesis (Altmann
1967; Deputte 1994) states that yawning as well
as self-scratching, a self-directed behavior
predicting anxiety in mammals (Palagi et al.
2019), rises after agonistic events, suggesting
a connection of yawning to anxiety
(Melopsittacus undulatus: Miller et al. 2010;
Rattus norvegicus:Moyaho and Valencia 2002;
Otaria flavescens:Palagi et al. 2019; Lemur
catta and Propithecus verreauxi: Zannella et al.
2015; and Theropithecus gelada: Leone et al.
2014).
-
- Although yawning has been extensively
studied in primate and carnivore species,
literature on ungulates is deficient (Baenninger
1997; Gallup 2011). Domestic horses (Equus ferus
caballus) are a rare case where yawning has been
described (Fureix et al. 2011). The authors
found that horses exhibit more yawning and
stereotypical behaviors (e.g., Lip play, head
shaking and nodding, weaving) in pre-feeding
than during/after feeding, suggesting a possible
linkage between yawning and anxiety also in
horses. In Przewalski horses (Equus ferus
przewalskii), a study revealed an association
between yawning and aggressive behavior
especially in bachelor males that frequently
engage in agonistic encounters. Additionally,
stallions yawned more than adult females and
immature males (Górecka-Bruzda et al.
2016). The authors suggest that stallions'
yawning can be triggered by higher testosterone
levels and social stress deriving from male-male
interactions.
-
- Despite the fixed nature of yawning, data
exist underlining the presence of a certain
degree of variability (Walusinski and Deputte
2004; Guggisberg et al. 2010; Vick and Paukner
2010; Provine 2012; Leone et al. 2014; Gallup et
al. 2016; Massen et al. 2021). In primates,
larger yawns (also called "threat yawns,"
Altmann 1967) are expressed more by males than
females and occur under high-tension situations
(Leone et al. 2014). Moreover, especially in
species with high sexual dimorphism, males
exhibit larger yawns during territorial defence
or competition for females (Zannella et al.
2015, 2017). In primate species, the diverse
yawn morphologies, defined based on teeth
visibility, are distributed differently as a
function of the social context (Pan
troglodytes:Vick and Paukner 2010; Theropithecus
gelada:Palagi et al. 2009; Macaca sp.: Zannella
et al. 2021).
-
- Despite the widespread presence of
spontaneous yawning across vertebrates (Heusner
1946; Baenninger 1997; Massen et al. 2021),
contagious yawning (CY) (the response to others'
yawns with a yawn) seems to be present in highly
cohesive and social species (Guggisberg et
al.2010). CY is described as a response to an
innate releasing mechanism (Provine and Hamernik
1986; Bartholomew and Cirulli 2014) and it has
been observed between individuals of the same
species (e.g., ungulates: Yonezawa et al. 2017;
Norscia et al. 2021; primates: Palagi et al.
2009; Demuru and Palagi 2012; van Berlo et al.
2020; carnivores: Romero et al.2014;
Wojczulanis-Jakubas et al. 2019; Casetta et al.
2021, 2022; Ake and Kutsukake 2023; birds:
Miller et al. 2012) or different species
(proboscidates: Rossman et al. 2020; primates:
Cambpell and de Waal 2014; Gallup and Wozny
2022; Pedruzzi et al. 2022; carnivores: Romero
et al.2013). One study on horses (Equus
caballus) provided no evidence for CY in this
species (Malavasi 2014).
-
- Although the issue is highly debated and far
to be completely demonstrated (Massen et al.
2012; Massen and Gallup 2017; Neilands et al.
2020), different authors suggest that the
phenomenon of CY could be influenced by social
factors (Social Modulation hypothesis): the
better the relationship quality of the subjects,
the higher their reciprocal susceptibility to CY
and shorter response latency (Romero et al.
2014). Most of the data about the Social
Modulation hypothesis come from primate species
such as redcapped mangabeys (Pedruzzi et al.
2022), geladas (Palagi et al. 2009; Pedruzzi et
al. 2022), chimpanzees (Campbell and de Waal
2011, 2014), bonobos (Demuru and Palagi 2012),
and humans (Norscia and Palagi, 2011; Palagi et
al. 2014).
-
- Recent findings suggest that CY helps
synchronize group activities. In lions (Panthera
leo), yawning is a reliable indicator of the
imminent behavioral state changes. CY between
the 2 agents promotes their subsequent
behavioral alignment (Casetta et al. 2021).
Behavioural synchronization between group
members is crucial to social life and plays a
central role in maintaining inter-individual
cohesion (Engel and Lamprecht 1997; Gautrais et
al. 2007; King and Cowlishaw 2009). A study
based on a social network involving more than
100 feral horses living in a multi-level society
showed that subjects belonging to the same unit
synchronize their activities (e.g., resting or
moving) more than subjects belonging to
different unit (Maeda et al. 2021).
-
- Synchronisation also contributes to increase
efficiency in group vigilance (Pays et al. 2007;
Beauchamp 2015) thus reducing predation risk,
again especially in ungulates (Equus caballus:
Souris et al. 2007; Bos taurus:
Sárová et al. 2013).
- Here we focus on spontaneous and CY in
horses. As a first step, by analyzing
spontaneous yawns in female groups of horses on
pasture, we aim at investigating the possible
presence of different morphs of yawning and
which factors can influence their expression
(e.g., context, age, dominance rank). As a
second step, due to the horse social
cohesiveness and propensity to synchronize
activities and movements, we expect to find CY
in our groups (Hypothesis 1). Moreover, in line
with the Social Modulation hypothesis, we expect
that CY is more frequent and rapid (i.e.,
shorter response latency), among subjects acting
in a cohesive way thus sharing preferential
relationships (Hypothesis 2).
-
- Discussion
- Despite the highly stereotyped nature of
yawning, universally considered as an excellent
example of fixed action pattern, there are some
differences in the way this behavior can be
displayed (Provine 2005). It has already been
demonstrated in several primate species that
different morphs of yawn exist (Vick and Paukner
2010; Leone et al. 2014; Zannella et al. 2021;
Galotti et al. 2024). Yawning in herbivores is
still neglected with no studies on yawning in
ungulates investigating its variability. Via
EquiFACS we tested whether a priori distinction
of 2 different yawn morphologies based on teeth
visibility (Yawn Uncovered Teeth, LucT; Yawn
Covered Teeth, -cr, Supplementary Figure S1) was
reliable. By analyzing a subset of well-visible
yawns, our results confirm that horses show 2
distinct yawn morphologies (Figure 2A) as it has
been found in social primates (Vick and Paukner
2010; Zannella et al. 2021; Galotti et al.
2024).
-
- Through a network analysis (NetFACS) we
assessed the likelihood of each AU and AD being
present in each yawning event showing that Yuct
was associated with 9 AUs/ADs, while Yct with 4
AUs/ADs (Figure 2B). The total absence of AU
overlapping and the higher number of AUs
recruited during Yuct suggest that this form of
yawn is more complex than Ycr. In addition, our
results can have a direct application. When it
is difficult to obtain videos of good quality
due to challenging observational settings, teeth
visibility per se can be used to discriminate
different morphs of yawns, thus providing a
useful tool in yawning investigation.
-
- Yawning may have communicative functions
(Guggisberg et al. 2010) especially in those
species that live in large social groups and
with complex social dynamics, as horses (Maeda
et al. 2021). In this perspective, in primates,
different yawn morphologies have been found to
be associated with different social contexts
(Theropithecus gelada, Leone et al. 2014; Pan
troglodytes, Vick and Paukner 2010; Macaca
tonkeana, Zannella et al. 2017). Contrary to
other studies, we did not find any influence of
the imminent individual context (social or
solitary) on the emission of the different types
of yawns. This could be related to the
limitation of our study groups including only
adult females with their offspring. Contrary to
the agonistic encounters involving stallions
(Olczak and Klocek 2014), female agonistic
interactions mostly involve threatening actions
probably provoking less anxiety variations in
animals. Indeed, a study on Przewalski horses
showed that after agonistic interactions,
stallions yawned more than females
(Górecka-Bruzda et al. 2016).
-
- If the context did not apparently influence
the different yawn morphologies, the breed seems
to have a role, with ponies of Monterufoli
performing more Yuct compared to Italian Heavy
Draft horses (Figure 3). Although, the
interpretation of this outcome is anything but
straightforward, a possible explanation can be
found in the Brain Cooling Hypothesis (Gallup et
al. 2016; Massen et al. 2021). We could
hypothesize that to cool a relatively larger
brain, the animal can display larger yawns
(Galotti et al. 2024). Shetland ponies have been
observed to have a number of distinctive cranial
features, including a broad skull with a rounded
ribcage, a concave face, and large orbits, all
characteristics predictive of a larger brain
compared to other breeds of horses (Hanot et al.
2021). These traits, that are typical of the
juvenile phases in horses, are retained in pony
adults probably due to a pedomorphosis
phenomenon (Goodwin et al. 2008). In short,
ponies manifest larger skulls compared with the
body size even as adults (Hanot et al. 2021).
The higher frequency of Yuct displayed by the
pony of Monterufoli could be an indirect cue
supporting the Brain Cooling Hypothesis (Gallup
et al. 2016; Massen et al. 2021), although it
remains highly speculative.
-
- Some authors state that the social value of
yawning is a derived feature, while the
primitive function of yawning is mainly
physiological (Gallup 2011; Palagi et al. 2020).
One of the most relevant results of our study is
the demonstration of CY in horses (Figure 4).
Since CY can have a role in social and motor
coordination (Casetta et al. 2021; Gallup 2022),
our finding fits with the high level of group
cohesion of this species (Maeda et al. 2021).
The possible role of CY in favoring
inter-individual cohesiveness is supported by
the data showing that our mares were more likely
to respond to kin compared to non-kin (Figure
5A) and to groupmates with whom they frequently
engaged in mutual grooming (Figure 5B). On the
other side, CY was not affected by the mares'
dominance rank. Females remain in their natal
groups throughout their entire lives allowing
them to form long-term bonds resulting in
coordinated movements during their daily
activities and active participation in
affiliative exchanges, such as mutual grooming
(Arnold and Grassia 1982; Waring 1983; Keiper
and Sambraus 1986). Among mares, dominance ranks
do not shape mutual grooming thus making the
mares' society highly fluid from a social
viewpoint (Kimura 1998). Hence, the social
modulation of CY reflects the sociobiology of
the species with a possible role to increase
synchronization of social activities that do not
seem to be mediated by dominance
relationships.
-
- Although some empirical data indicate that
CY helps synchronize activities and not
viceversa (lions, Casetta et al. 2021; humans,
Gallup and Meyers 2021), the potential impact of
circadian rhythms on the synchronization of
yawns cannot be discounted (Massen et al. 2017).
The tendency of social animals to engage in
shared activities, such as waking, feeding, and
resting, leads to the alignment of their
circadian rhythms. It is a reasonable hypothesis
that spontaneous yawns cluster temporally within
social groups, due to the alignment of their
circadian rhythms. This could explain why yawns
occur in rapid succession and, unlike chewing
events, may be attributed to both contagion and
common neurophysiological changes, such as
fluctuations in brain or body temperature
(Landolt et al. 1995). Additionally, kinship and
grooming can facilitate the synchronization
among socially bonded animals (Massen and Gallup
2017; Gallup 2022).
-
- To promptly synchronize their activities,
humans and non-human animals need to rapidly
change their behavior according to others'
(Nagasaka et al. 2013; Hattori 2021). CY was
faster in mares that were kin compared to
non-kin (Figure 6A) and reached the lowest
latency response between kin mares who mostly
groomed each other (Figure 6B). disen Data on
humans (Gallup and Meyers 2021) and social
carnivores (Casetta et al. 2021) indicate that
perceiving a yawn from a conspecific increases
the alertness state in the receiver that will be
more prone to respond in a congruent and rapid
way. From an evolutionary viewpoint, the ability
to respond more and faster to familiar yawns can
provide selective advantages due to an increased
behavioral coordination. This hypothesis needs
to be addressed in future studies where the
temporal relation between the CY event and the
subsequent behavioral alignment of the 2
interacting agents should be evaluated. We are
confident that expanding the exploration of
yawning to other ungulate social species living
in complex societies can open new scenarios on
the functions of spontaneous yawning, CY, and
their predictable immediate consequences for the
social partners.
|
|