mystery of yawning
Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
http://www.baillement.com
 
 
 
 

mise à jour du
31 août 2012
 
 
Scholarpedia
Yawning
in
Encyclopedia of Sleep and Dreams:
The Evolution, Function, Nature, and Mysteries of Slumber
published by ABC-CLIO

Chat-logomini

Fetal Yawning
in Sonography Edited by Kerry Thoirs
ISBN 978-953-307-947-9, Hard cover, 346 pages Publisher: InTech
 
The yawn is a stereotyped and often repetitive motor act characterized by gaping of the mouth accompanied by a long inspiration of breath, a brief acme, and then a short expiration of breath. Stretching and yawning simultaneously is known as pandiculation. It is not merely a simple opening of the mouth but a complex coordinated movement bringing together a flexion followed by an extension of the neck, a wide dilatation of the pharyngolarynx with strong stretching of the diaphragm and antigravity muscles.
 
Ethologists agree that almost all vertebrates yawn. Yawning is morphologically similar in reptiles, birds, mammals, and fish. These behaviors may be ancestral vestiges maintained throughout evolution, with little variation (phylogenetic old origins). Correlatively, yawning can be visualized as early as 12 weeks during the period of fetal development.
 
Systematic and coordinated pandiculations occur in a similar pattern and form across all animals, and consistently occur during behaviors associated with cyclic life rhythms: sleep arousal, feeding, and reproduction. Yawning appears as one undirected response to an inner stimulation, underlying the homeostasis of these three behaviors.
 
Species that sleep 8 to 12 hours and alternate between active and inactive periods (e.g., predatory carnivores and primates) yawn much more frequently (following a circadian rhythm) than herbivores. In humans, daily frequency of yawning varies between 5 and 15 times per day. The diurnal distribution of yawning frequency is illustrated by higher frequency upon waking and before sleep.
 
Neurophysiology
A good number of clinical and pharmacological data indicate that yawning involves a group of oxytocinergic neurons originating in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN), and projecting to extrahypothalamic brain areas (e.g., hippocampus, medulla oblongata, and spinal cord). The PVN is an integration center between the central and peripheral autonomic nervous systems. It is involved in a number of functions ranging from feeding and metabolic balance to sexual behavior and yawning. Activation of these neurons by dopamine and its agonists, excitatory amino acids (N-methyl-D-aspartic acid), oxytocin itself, or by electrical stimulation leads to yawning; conversely their inhibition by gamma-aminobutyric acid and its agonists or by opioid peptides and opiate-like drugs inhibit both yawning and sexual response. Other compounds modulate yawning by activating central oxytocinergic neurons: sexual hormones, serotonin, hypocretin, and endogenous peptides (adrenocorticotropin-melanocytestimulating hormone). Oxytocin activates cholinergic neurotransmission in the hippocampus and the reticular formation of the brainstem. Acetylcholine induces yawning via the muscarinic receptors of effectors from which the respiratory neurons in the medulla; the motor nuclei of the 5th,7th, 9th, 10th, and 12th cranial nerves; the phrenic nerves (C1-C4); and the motor supply to the intercostal muscles.
 
Contagiousness of Yawning
It seems that hominids have the unique capacity to be receptive to the contagiousness of yawning. In humans, echokinesis only occurs in situations of minimal mental stimulation (public transport, waiting); people are not susceptible to this phenomenon during prolonged intellectual effort.
 
Yawning appears to trigger a sort of social coordination function and reflects the capacity to unconsciously and automatically be influenced by the behavior of others. Autistic individuals who are characterized by impaired mental state attribution do not show contagious yawning. All these data support the hypothesis that contagious yawning shares the neural networks implicated in self-recognition and mental state attribution; it may therefore be that yawning is involved in empathy.
 
Yawning in diseases
Excessive yawning is a source of embarrassment in social circles. There are multiple causes of excessive yawning, that is, a cluster of 10 to 50 yawns, many times a day. Of short duration, they may predict a vasovagal reaction or neurovegetative disorders (dyspepsia, migraine-like syndromes). All insults to the intracranial central nervous system or the hypothalamo-hypophyseal region may be involved: tumors with intracranial hypertension, infections, temporal epilepsy, strokes, etc. Actually, iatrogenic pathology (serotoninergic agents, apomorphine, acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, opiate withdrawal) is the most frequent explanation of pathologic cases. Excessive yawning may also predict a sleep apnea syndrome with sleepiness.
 
Why do we yawn ? The hypothesis
Yawning does not accelerate blood flow. This blood-flow theory argued that yawning improved the oxygenation of the brain, in response to cerebral anemia. The inaccuracy of this hypothesis was formally invalidated by Provine, Tate, and Geldmacher (1987). In his studies, he has demonstrated that breathing neither pure O2 nor gases high in CO2 had any significant effect on yawning, although both increased breathing rate. In a second study, he has found that exercise sufficient to double breathing rate had no effect on yawning. Although the available data are far from providing a complete and generally accepted account of the physiological function of yawning, progress has been made in ruling out previously held hypotheses.
 
Conclusion
Yawning and pandiculation are transitional behaviors, universal among vertebrates, closer to an emotional stereotypy than a reflex. Phylogenetically ancient and ontogenetically primitive, they may provide some evolutionary advantage. They seem to exteriorize homeostatic processes of systems controlling wakefulness, satiety, and sexuality in the diencephalon.
 
oxytocin yawning
References
 
Barbizet, J. (1958). Yawning. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 21(3), 203-209.
 
Collins, G. T., & Eguibar, J. R. (2010). Neurophamacology of yawning. Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience, 28, 90-106.
 
Deputte, B. L. (1974). Revue sur le comportement de bâillement chez les vertébrés. Bulletin interne Société Française pour l'étude du comportement animal, 1, 26-35.
 
Guggisberg, A. G., Mathis, J., Schnider, A., & Hess, C. W. (2010). Why do we yawn? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 1267-1276.
 
Nahab, F. B. (2010). Exploring yawning with neuroimaging. Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience, 28, 128-133.
 
Platek, S. M., Mohamed, F. B., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2005). Contagious yawning and the brain. Cognitive Brain Research, 23, 448-452.
 
Provine, R. R., Tate, B. C., & Geldmacher, L. L. (1987). Yawning: No effect of 3-5% CO2, 100% O2, and exercise. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 48(3), 382-393. 811