Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
http://www.baillement.com

mystery of yawning 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

haut de page

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

mise à jour du
14 février 2022
Am J Primatol. 2022:e23366
Yawn contagion in bonobos:
another group, another story
Norscia I, Caselli M, De Meo G,
Cordoni G, Guéry JP, Demuru E.

Chat-logomini

Tous les articles sur la contagion du bâillement
All articles about contagious yawning
 
 
In primates, yawn contagion (the yawning response elicited by others' yawn) is variably influenced by individual (e.g., sex, age) and social factors (e.g., familiarity) and possibly linked to interindividual synchronization, coordination, and emotional contagion. Two out of three studies on yawn contagion in bonobos (Pan paniscus), found the presence of the phenomenon with mixed results concerning the effect of familiarity and no replication on its modulating factors. To address this puzzling issue, the authors recorded all occurrences data on yawn contagion in a captive bonobo group (March-June 2021; 18 individuals; La Vallée des Singes, France). Contrary to chimpanzees and humans, the number of triggering yawns increased contagion, possibly owing to a higher stimulus threshold. This aspect may explain the interindividual variability observed in yawn contagion rates. In subjects under weaning, they did not detect yawn contagion and, as it occurs in certain human cohorts, yawn contagion declined with age, possibly due to reduced sensitivity to others. Females responded more than males and elicited more responses from females when showing sexual swelling. As reproductive females are central in bonobo society, our results support the hypothesis that-as in other Hominini-the most influential sex can influence yawn contagion. The relationship quality (measured via grooming/play) did not affect yawn contagion, possibly due to bonobos' xenophilic nature. Overall, this study confirms the presence of yawn contagion in bonobos and introduces new elements on its modulating factors, pointing toward the necessity of cross-group studies.
 
 
Chez les primates, la contagion du bâillement (la réponse au bâillement provoquée par le bâillement des autres) est influencée de manière variable par des facteurs individuels (par exemple, le sexe, l'âge) et sociaux (par exemple, la familiarité) et peut-être liée à la synchronisation interindividuelle, la coordination et la contagion émotionnelle. Deux des trois études sur la contagion des bâillements chez les bonobos (Pan paniscus) ont révélé la présence du phénomène avec des résultats mitigés concernant l'effet de la familiarité et aucune confirmation de ses facteurs modulateurs.
 
Pour répondre à cette question, les auteurs ont enregistré toutes les occurrences de contagion des bâillements dans un groupe de bonobos en captivité (mars-juin 2021 ; 18 individus ; La Vallée des Singes, France). Contrairement aux chimpanzés et aux humains, le nombre de bâillements déclencheurs augmente la contagion, peut-être en raison d'un seuil de stimulus plus élevé. Cet aspect peut expliquer la variabilité interindividuelle observée dans les taux de contagion des bâillements. Chez les sujets en cours de sevrage, nous n'avons pas détecté de contagion des bâillements et, comme cela se produit dans certaines cohortes humaines, la contagion des bâillements a diminué avec l'âge, peut-être en raison d'une sensibilité réduite aux autres. Les femelles répondaient davantage que les mâles et suscitaient davantage de réponses de la part des femelles lorsqu'elles montraient un gonflement sexuel. Comme les femelles reproductrices jouent un rôle central dans la société des bonobos, nos résultats soutiennent l'hypothèse selon laquelle, comme chez les autres hominidés, le sexe est le plus influant peut moduler la contagion du bâillement. La qualité de la relation (mesurée par le toilettage/le jeu) n'a pas affecté la contagion du bâillement, peut-être en raison de la nature xénophile des bonobos. Dans l'ensemble, cette étude confirme la présence de la contagion du bâillement chez les bonobos et introduit de nouveaux éléments sur ses facteurs modulateurs, soulignant la nécessité d'études intergroupes.
INTRODUCTION
While spontaneous yawning is not dependent on the detection of others' yawns, contagious yawning occurs when the yawn emitted by an individual (hereafter trigger) works as a releasing stimulus (sensu Tinbergen & Perdeck, 1950) and induces yawning in another in- dividual (hereafter responder) (Provine, 1989). Spontaneous yawning (or a yawning_like morphological pattern) is likely a plesiomorphic display because it is present in a wide range of vertebrates (Baenninger, 1987), including human (Homo sapiens, Provine, 1986, 2012) and non_human primates (Anderson, 2020).
Contagious yawning between conspecifics is possibly an apo- morphic phenomenon described so far in a limited array of species (Palagi et al., 2020). From an adaptive point of view, yawn contagion can promote synchronization and coordination of activities within social groups (Palagi et al., 2020). Moreover, it can be the expression of interindividual physiological resonance (Prochazkova & Kret, 2017) and possibly emotional contagion, a powerful driver of prosocial behavior (de Waal & Preston, 2017).
Experimental and naturalistic studies on chimpanzees (e.g., Anderson et al., 2004; Campbell & Cox, 2019; Campbell & de Waal, 2011) and humans (e.g., Bartholomew & Cirulli, 2014; Chan & Tseng, 2017; Norscia & Palagi, 2011; Norscia et al., 2021a; Provine, 1986, 1989) have consistently found intraspecific yawn contagion. In bo- nobos the situation is not as much clear. Amici et al. (2014) examined whether yawning was subject to response facilitation triggered by videorecorded yawns from conspecifics. They found that chimpan- zees (14 subjects) but not bonobos (4 subjects) yawned significantly more while or after watching a familiar conspecific yawning on video. On the other hand, on a larger sample (25 subjects), Tan et al. (2017) found that bonobos showed evidence for involuntary, contagious yawning in response to videos of yawning conspecifics. Finally, Demuru and Palagi (2012) also reported yawn contagion in captive bonobos (12 subjects) based on ethological observations under nat- uralistic conditions. Hence, yawn contagion as a social signal might have been present in the last common ancestor between Pan and Homo.
Beyond Hominini, it is not possible to associate the emergence of yawn contagion with a single common ancestor. Yawn contagion was not detected in lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla; Amici et al., 2014; Palagi et al., 2019) but it was found in orangutans (Pongo spp.; van Berlo et al., 2020) which separated earlier from the human line (Groves, 2018). Interestingly, lowland gorillas show low affiliation levels (Palagi et al., 2019) whereas orangutans do not form social groups but orangutans might have been more social in the past (Harrison & Chivers, 2007). In non_hominid primates, yawn contagion studies show mixed results (cf. geladas, Theropithecus gelada, Gallo et al., 2021; Palagi et al., 2009; Tonkean macaque, Macaca tonkeana, Palagi & Norscia, 2019; but see: stump_tailed macaques, Macaca arctoides: Paukner & Anderson, 2006; Japanese macaque, Macaca fuscata, Palagi & Norscia, 2019). Finally, no evidence of yawn con- tagion was found in strepsirrhines (Lemur catta and Varecia variegata, Reddy et al., 2016) even though contagious yawning is present in non primates (Gallup et al., 2015; for review: Palagi et al., 2020). Hence, yawning might have been co_opted as a communicative signal mul- tiple times over the course of the evolution, in relation to the type of sociality.
 
When present, yawn contagion in primates usually occurs in the few minutes following the yawning stimulus (hereafter trig- gering yawn) with a peak in the first minute in Hominini (e.g., humans: Palagi et al., 2014; chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes: Campbell & Cox, 2019; and bonobos: Demuru & Palagi, 2012). In humans, perceptual factors may influence the yawning response probability (Massen & Gallup, 2017; Norscia et al., 2020). How- ever, the distance between trigger and responder and/or the number of observed triggering yawns were not found to affect yawn contagion (humans: Norscia & Palagi, 2011; chimpanzees: Campbell & Cox, 2019; geladas: Palagi et al., 2009).
 
Yawn contagion can be influenced by individual and social fac- tors (Palagi et al., 2020). The age of the responder can affect yawn contagion rates in some cohorts of humans (Anderson & Meno, 2003; Bartholomew & Cirulli, 2014; Helt et al., 2010; Hoogenhout et al., 2013) and chimpanzees (Madsen et al., 2013). No study so far has addressed this issue in bonobos. Moreover, in Hominini the yawning response can vary depending on the sex of the responder or the trigger. For example, women may respond more to others' yawns (Chan & Tseng, 2017; Norscia et al., 2016a, 2016b), although this does not occur in all cohorts (Bartholomew & Cirulli, 2014; Norscia & Palagi, 2011). Moreover, in the Pan genus yawning response can vary in relation to the trigger's sex, possibly depending on the social role that each sex has in different species (Demuru & Palagi, 2012; Massen & Gallup, 2017). Finally, yawn contagion was found to be influenced by the level of familiarity between subjects in humans (Norscia & Palagi, 2011; Norscia et al., 2020), chimpanzees (Campbell & de Waal, 2011), and in one out of two groups of bonobos (cf. Demuru & Palagi, 2012; Tan et al., 2017), with highest yawn con- tagion rates being recorded between particularly familiar subjects.
 
In sum, two out of the three independent studies on the presence of yawn contagion in bonobos detected the phenomenon (cf. Amici et al., 2014; Demuru & Palagi, 2012; Tan et al., 2017) and yawn contagion was higher between closely bonded (compared to weakly bonded) group mates (Demuru & Palagi, 2012) but not between group mates when compared to non_group mates (Tan et al., 2017). To better understand the phenomenon, we investigated yawn contagion in yet another group of bonobos. We formulated the following predictions.
Prediction 1: Presence and distribution of yawn contagion. Based on previous findings on the presence of yawn contagion in two bonobo groups (Demuru & Palagi, 2012; Tan et al., 2017), we expected to find the phenomenon also in our study group (Prediction 1a). Demuru and Palagi (2012) found the maximum yawn contagion rates in the first minute after the triggering stimulus. Hence, we expected to find a similar result in our study group (Prediction 1b). Because yawn contagion was not found in all bonobos (Amici et al., 2014), we expected to find a high contagion variability across subjects (Prediction 1c).
 
Prediction 2: Perceptual factors&emdash;Possibly due to the high visual acuity of anthropoid primates (Fleagle, 2013), the spatial distance from trigger and responder was found to have no effect on yawn contagion in chimpanzees (Campbell & Cox, 2019) and geladas (Palagi et al., 2009). Hence, we expected to find no in- fluence of trigger_responder distance on yawn contagion in bonobos (Prediction 2a). Moreover, in humans and chimpanzees observing several yawns in a row does not seem to raise the chance of yawn contagion (humans: Norscia & Palagi, 2011; chimpanzees: Campbell & Cox, 2019). Hence, we expected a similar result in bonobos owing to their phylogenetic closeness with humans and chimpanzees (Prediction 2b).
 
Prediction 3: Individual and social factors. In the Hominini, the trigger's rank and sex can have an influence on yawn contagion rates, with individuals responding mostly to men in certain cohorts of hu- mans (for yawns that are heard but not seen; Norscia et al., 2020) and chimpanzees (dominant males especially; Massen & Gallup, 2017) and to females in bonobos (Demuru & Palagi, 2012). While males are central in chimpanzee dominance relationships (Bray et al., 2021; Lewis et al., 2021), in bonobos reproductive females are central in determining group dynamics (e.g., Furuichi, 2011). Hence, we expected that trigger's rank and sex, especially adult females, could play a major role in eliciting the yawning response (Prediction 3a). As concerns the effect of age, no study on bonobos has addressed this factor on yawn contagion so far. However, age appears to have an effect in humans (Anderson & Meno, 2003; Bartholomew a& Cirulli, 2014; Helt et al., 2010) and in chimpanzees (Madsen et al., 2013), with yawn contagion being higher in adults than in immature sub- jects. In certain cohorts of adult humans, yawning decreases with aging (Bartholomew & Cirulli, 2014). This aspect has not been in- vestigated in chimpanzees. Owing to the phylogenetic closeness of bonobos to humans and chimpanzees (Pru_fer et al., 2012), we expected that age might have a similar effect on yawn contagion in our study group (Prediction 3b). In humans and chimpanzees, familiarity between individuals has been reported to increase yawn contagion rates (humans: Norscia & Palagi, 2011; Norscia et al., 2016a; chimpanzees: Campbell & de Waal, 2011). In bonobos, no familiarity effect was found between non_group members in an experimental setting (using video trials; Tan et al., 2017) but it was found within known subjects in naturalistic conditions, with yawn contagion being highest between closely bonded group mates (Demuru & Palagi, 2012). Thus, we expected to find a positive effect of familiarity on yawn contagion in our bonobo group, observed under naturalistic conditions (Prediction 3c).
 
 
DISCUSSION
Presence of yawn contagion
Yawn contagion was present in our study group because it was more likely that bonobos yawned after seeing a yawn (PY condition) compared to when they did not observe any previous yawn (MC condition; Prediction 1a supported; Figure 3). Hence, yawn contagion may be present at the population level, as it has been found so far in three different groups (present study; Demuru & Palagi, 2012; Tan et al., 2017). Yawn contagion is also present in different cohorts of other Hominini (chimpanzees: Anderson et al., 2004; Campbell & Cox, 2019; Campbell & de Waal, 2011; humans: e.g., Bartholomew & Cirulli, 2014; Chan & Tseng, 2017; Cordoni et al., 2021; Norscia & Palagi, 2011; Provine, 1989) and, as a form of autonomic contagion, can increase interindividual synchronization and coordination (Casetta et al., 2021; de Waal & Preston, 2017; Prochazkova & Kret, 2017).
 
We found that the phenomenon was present only in the first minute after the yawn stimulus (Prediction 1b supported), when we detected a significant difference between PY and MC conditions (Figure 3b). On one hand, this result is in line with previous reports showing a peak of yawn contagion in the first minute, compared to following minutes, in bonobos (Demuru & Palagi, 2012) and the other Hominini (chimpanzees: Campbell & Cox, 2019; humans: Palagi et al., 2014). On the other hand, our result introduces an element of novelty because it shows that yawn contagion occurred only (not just maximally) in the first minute (or up to the second minute, if we consider the nonsignificant trend as the basis for further investigation).
 
In our naturalistic study we found significant variability in the ICTs across subjects (Prediction 2c supported) and one adult male did not show contagion (yawning more in MC than in PY condition). Even though previous studies did not specifically focus on yawn contagion interindividual variation (Amici et al., 2014; Demuru & Palagi, 2012; Tan et al., 2017), Amici et al. (2014) found that in four bonobos yawning was not triggered by video stimuli of yawning conspecifics. Interindividual variability may explain at least in part why contagion is not expressed in all subjects. In healthy humans, 40%&endash;60% of sub- jects did not show yawn contagion under laboratory conditions (Platek et al., 2003; Provine, 1986, 1989) and susceptibility to others' yawns appears to be stable across contexts (Bartholomew & Cirulli, 2014). Analogously in bonobos, yawn contagion can be context in- dependent (e.g. resting/relaxing vs social tension contexts; Demuru & Palagi, 2012). Future studies on interindividual fluctuations can shed light on within_population variability.
 
Perceptual factors affecting yawn contagion
The spatial distance between trigger and responder had no significant effect on yawn contagion (Table 1; Prediction 2a confirmed). Con- sistently, no influence of trigger_responder distance was found in chimpanzees (Campbell & Cox, 2019) and geladas (Palagi et al., 2009). This is not surprising because anthropoid primates possess high visual acuity and mainly rely on stereoscopic vision to orient themselves in the world (Fleagle, 2013).
 
In our study group, yawn contagion probability increased as the number of triggering yawns increased (Prediction 2b not supported; Figure 4a; Table 1). This is in contrast with the situation found in humans and chimpanzees, in which no such effect was found (Campbell & Cox, 2019; Norscia & Palagi, 2011). Interestingly, Norscia et al. (2021b) found that in domestic pigs both trigger_ responder spatial distance and the number of (non_vocalized) yawning stimuli affected yawn contagion rates possibly due to the scarce visual acuity of the species. It is possible that bonobos, compared to humans, possess a higher yawn contagion threshold and that the yawning response is most likely primed after observing multiple yawns. This possibility may contribute to the interindividual variability observed in bonobo yawn contagion and might point to- wards possible neurobiological differences in stimulus processing. Future cross_species studies are necessary to clarify this issue.
 
4.3 | Individual and social factors modulating yawn contagion
Compared to males, females were not overall more effective as triggers even though a previous study fount that adult females ten- ded to induce others' yawns more than males (Demuru & Palagi, 2012). This difference may be due to the fact that our female sample included females with and without a swelling cycle, which allowed us to test for this variable (not tested before). We found that females with a swelling cycle elicited more yawning responses from other females compared to females without swelling cycle (Table 1 and Figure 4d). In this respect, Prediction 3a can be at least partially confirmed. Swelling in bonobos is an important communicative signal not just for males but also for females (Demuru et al., 2020) and can contribute to determining their social status by favoring female_ female socio_sexual interactions and alliances (Furuichi, 2011; Moscovice et al., 2019). Analogously, in chimpanzees _ in which males form alliances to control resources (Bray et al., 2021; Lewis et al., 2021)&emdash;males seem to be most powerful in eliciting yawn contagion, especially if dominant (Massen & Gallup, 2017). Rank per se had no significant influence on yawn contagion in bonobos possibly due to the high tolerance level of the species (Furuichi, 2011; Hare & Kwetuenda, 2010). Indeed, in our bonobo group hierarchy showed relatively low steepness, which indicates rather shallow hierarchy. Interestingly, females showed the highest yawn contagion rates (Table 1 and Figure 4b), which may related to their central role in bonobo groups. Such a role may require an enhanced sensitivity to social signals, such as yawning, which may favor interindividual syn- chronization and social cohesion. In humans, an increased yawning response of women has been observed in some cases (Chan & Tseng, 2017; Norscia et al., 2016a) but not in others (Bartholomew & Cirulli, 2014; Norscia & Palagi, 2011). The socio_cultural influence characterizing different human cohorts makes it hard to single out an unambiguous effect of gender on yawn contagion (Palagi et al., 2020).
 
We detected no yawn contagion (as responders) in the two infants (aged 4 months and 4 years old) and our statistical analysis on subadults and adults showed that yawn contagion decreased with age (Table 1 and Figure 4c; Prediction 3b confirmed). The responder's age seems to affect yawn contagion also in other Hominini. In chimpanzees yawn contagion was found in adult subjects, but absent in infant subjects (Madsen et al., 2013). In humans, yawn contagion is absent, reduced or differently age_modulated in infants (Anderson & Meno, 2003; Cordoni et al., 2021; Helt et al., 2010; Millen & Anderson, 2011). In human and non_human mammals, the increase of yawn contagion with age (especially from the immature phase to adulthood) has been associated with possible maturation of socio_ cognitive abilities and/or neural pathways that decode social cues and with the ontogenetic variation in the ability to identify the in- ternal states of others (Cordoni et al., 2021; Madsen & Persson, 2013; Norscia et al., 2021b).
 
In certain human cohorts, yawn contagion can decline with age (over 40; Bartholomew & Cirulli, 2014) possibly due to a decreased sensitivity to others' states (Palagi et al., 2020). Yawn contagion, possibly mediated by bottom_up cognitive processes (Palagi et al., 2020), might also decrease with age as the result of the increased top_down mechanisms in emotional processing. Interestingly, in humans aging seems to be associated with a switch from bottom_up to top_down processes in emotion appraisal (Petro et al., 2021; Reed & Carstensen, 2012). Further neuroethological studies are necessary to verify these hypotheses.
 
Finally, the affiliation levels between group mates (a social attachment indicator; Dunbar, 1991) did not affect the likelihood of yawn contagion (Table 1; Prediction 3c not confirmed). Social attachment (informed by affiliation levels, kinship and/or group membership) can increase yawn contagion rates (Palagi et al., 2020). Such effect has been observed in humans (Norscia & Palagi, 2011; Norscia et al., 2016a), chimpanzees (Campbell & de Waal, 2011) and other mammals (e.g., domestic pigs, Norscia et al., 2021b; wolves, Romero et al., 2014). The presence of the so_called 'familiarity bias' suggests that emotional contagion may influence the phenomenon of yawn contagion (de Waal & Preston, 2017). In bonobos, the situation is puzzling because no effect of group membership (group vs. non_group members) was experimentally found in one group (Tan et al., 2017) whereas a positive effect of social bond between group mates was found in another group via a naturalistic approach (affiliation rates and kinship were combined; Demuru & Palagi, 2012). At the very proximate level, the familiarity bias on yawn contagion may be dampened in our study colony by the fact that individuals had been together in the same group&emdash;with no fission_ fusion management&emdash;for a long time (min&endash;max range: 4&endash;12 years). Affiliation rates occurring in the short term may not reliably inform on long_term familiarity. At the ultimate level, the xenophilic nature of bonobos (showing affiliation between group residents and non_ residents, high intergroup tolerance and food sharing with strangers; Furuichi, 2011; Idani, 1991; Lucchesi et al., 2020; Tan & Hare, 2013; Tan et al., 2017) may have contributed to reducing the adaptive value of familiarity. The lack of familiarity bias was also found in an opposite situation. Particularly, van Berlo et al. (2020) found the presence of yawn contagion in captive orangutans with no effect of familiarity was detected. Wild orangutans do not live in social groups but show dispersed sociality (with occasional en- counters). Here, the effect of familiarity may have a reduced adaptive significance because individuals do not form preferential social bonds or alliances. The opposite cases of bonobos (Demuru & Palagi, 2012; Tan et al., 2017; present study) and orangutans (van Berlo et al., 2020) converge in indicating that the familiarity bias may be related to interindividual cohesion (proximate level) and type of sociality (ultimate level). In contrast with previous reports (Joly_Mascheroni et al., 2008; Romero et al., 2013; Silva et al., 2012), a meta_analysis showed that familiarity seems not to affect interspecific yawn contagion between dogs and humans (Neilands et al., 2020). A similar approach could help disentangle the familiarity issue in bonobos, especially if by including data collected with the same methodologies on different colonies. Once again, owing to the differences observed across study groups and sites, we stress the importance of expanding the dataset on yawn contagion to account for intergroup differences and clarify what factors can modulate the phenomenon at the population level.