Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
Le bâillement, du réflexe à la pathologie
Le bâillement : de l'éthologie à la médecine clinique
Le bâillement : phylogenèse, éthologie, nosogénie
 Le bâillement : un comportement universel
La parakinésie brachiale oscitante
Yawning: its cycle, its role
Warum gähnen wir ?
 
Fetal yawning assessed by 3D and 4D sonography
Le bâillement foetal
http://www.baillement.com

mystery of yawning 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

haut de page

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

mise à jour du
1 octobre 2014
Dysphagia
2015;30(1):57-66
Occurrences of Yawn and Swallow
are Temporally Related
Abe Kimikos, Weisz SE, Dunn RL, DiGioacchino MC, Nyentap JA,
Stanbouly S, Theurer JA, Bureau Y, Affoo RH, Martin RE.
 
School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Faculty of Health Sciences,
Western University, London, Ontario, Canada
 
Air swallowing as a tic Weil et al.

Chat-logomini

Electrophysiological association of spontaneous yawning and swallowing. Ertekin C, Bulbul NG, Uludag IF, et al 2015
 
Abstract
 
Yawning is a stereotyped motor behavior characterized by deep inhalation and associated dilation of the respiratory tract, pronounced jaw opening, and facial grimacing. The frequency of spontaneous yawning varies over the diurnal cycle, peaking after waking and before sleep. Yawning can also be elicited by seeing or hearing another yawn, or by thinking about yawning, a phenomenon known as "contagious yawning". Yawning is mediated by a distributed network of brainstem and supratentorial brain regions, the components of which are shared with other airway behaviors including respiration, swallowing, and mastication. Nevertheless, the possibility of behavioral coordination between yawning and other brainstem-mediated functions has not been examined. Here we show, with a double-blind methodology, a greater-than-fivefold increase in rest (saliva) swallowing rate during the 10-s period immediately following contagious yawning elicited in 14 adult humans through the viewing of videotaped yawn stimuli. Sixty-five percent of yawns were followed by a swallow within 10 s and swallows accounted for 26 % of all behaviors produced during this post-yawn period. This novel finding of a tight temporal coupling between yawning and swallowing provides preliminary evidence that yawning and swallowing are physiologically related, thus extending current models of upper airway physiology and neurophysiology. Moreover, our finding suggests the possibility that yawning plays a role in eliciting rest swallowing, a view not considered in previous theories of yawning. As such, the present demonstration of a temporal association between yawning and swallowing motivates a re-examination of the longstanding question, "Why do we yawn?".
Introduction
 
Yawning is a stereotyped motor behavior observed in a variety of vertebrate species including non-human and human primates [1&endash;3]. A yawn can be conceptualized as a paroxysmal sequence of muscle contractions that gives rise to three phases of yawn-related movements over a period of 4&endash;7 s [1, 3]. The initial phase of yawning is characterized by gradual jaw opening and retraction and lowering of the tongue and hyoid bone [1]. This series of movements is accompanied by contraction of the diaphragm causing inspiration and associated dilation of the pharynx, larynx, trachea, and bronchi. During the second phase of yawning, continued inspiration increases thoracic volume, with associated continued dilation of the pharynx, larynx, and thorax. There is maximal jaw opening, facial grimacing, and extension of the neck, trunk, and sometimes the limbs. The third phase of yawning is passive and involves a transition from inspiration to expiration, possibly accompanied by vocalization, and the return of the orofacial and thoracic musculature to its rest position. Thus, yawning produces a generalized and pronounced stretching of the respiratory, axial and limb musculoskeletal systems [1, 4, 5].
 
The frequency of spontaneous yawning varies over the diurnal cycle, with the highest frequencies observed at waking and before sleep [6&endash;8]. In humans, the daily frequency of spontaneous yawning has been reported to average from 8.7 [9] to 13.46 [6], with a range of 0&endash;28 [9]. Additionally, in certain species, ''contagious yawning'' can be elicited by experiencing another yawn or, in the case of humans, by simply thinking about yawning [3, 10]. While the mechanisms underlying contagious yawning remain unclear, brain-imaging studies have implicated brain regions that process motor imitation, empathy and social behavior, including the mirror neuron system [11&endash;15]. The phenomenology of yawning has fuelled divergent hypotheses regarding its functional significance. In particular, the distinction between spontaneous yawning and contagious yawning has led to the view that yawning may have both physiological and social roles.
 
Physiological hypotheses propose that yawning regulates a peripheral organ system and/or a central nervous system (CNS) function such as respiratory homeostasis or CNS arousal [14]. For example, Walusinski [16] reported that the strong sequence of muscle contractions in yawning disrupts the functional neural network mediating rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep and associated motor patterns, and facilitates the emergence of another functional network that controls motor patterns of awakening. In contrast, contagious yawning has been linked to empathy and social proximity to conspecifics. In support of this proposal, humans who perform better on self-recognition and theory-of-mind tasks, which are believed to reflect the capacity for empathy, demonstrate a greater propensity to yawn in response to a contagion [12]. Contagious yawning in chimpanzees has been shown to reflect social ''ingroup'' versus ''outgroup'' biases [17].
 
Nonetheless, the functional significance of yawning remains unclear. Lesion studies in patients and, more specifically, neuropharmacological studies in which yawning has been evoked through the application of neurochemical agents to various sites within the CNS in animal models, have suggested that yawning is under the control of several neurotransmitters and neuropeptides that act on a distributed network of brainstem and supratentorial regions including the medulla and pons, the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) of the hypothalamus, and cortical and subcortical regions, with the PVN possibly playing a fundamental role in initiating yawning [1, 18, 19]. Muscles that contract during yawning are innervated by cranial nerves V, VII, IX, X, XI, XII, cervical nerves C1&endash;C4, and dorsal nerves innervating the intercostals [20]. These brainstem circuits are also involved in the sensorimotor control of other upper airway behaviors including respiration, swallowing, and mastication [21, 22]. This shared neural circuitry suggests the possibility that yawning is coupled with other brainstemmediated functions. Nevertheless, the possibility of behavioral coordination between yawning and other upper airway functions has not been examined. Our laboratory and others have reported a series of studies showing that stimulation of the posterior oral cavity
 
and oropharynx with air-pulse trains is associated with a statistically significant increase in the frequency of resting (i.e., saliva) swallowing [23]. This up-regulation of swallowing has been demonstrated in young and older healthy adults, and in patients with swallowing impairment secondary to stroke [24, 25]. Interestingly, among individuals who had suffered a stroke, the air-pulse trains were, in a small number of instances, followed initially by a yawn followed by swallowing (Theurer, unpublished observations). This unanticipated observation is consistent with the view that yawning and rest swallowing may be physiologically related. Therefore, the purpose of the current study was to examine the temporal relationship between contagious yawning and rest swallowing. It was hypothesized that rest swallowing rate would be increased during the period of time immediately following yawning, compared with other periods during which yawning did not occur. A temporal coupling between yawning and rest swallowing would be expected to have clinical significance insofar as methods to elicit contagious yawning might be employed in rehabilitation to evoke swallowing, for example, in individuals with dysphagia.
 
Discussion
 
Elicitation of Yawning
 
Based on previous evidence that yawning frequency increases in association with the viewing of yawning, the present study employed a yawn video to elicit contagious yawning among the study participants [6, 12, 26]. As anticipated, the frequency of yawning increased significantly during viewing of the yawn video, supporting the efficacy of this visual stimulus in promoting contagious yawning. Nevertheless, the degree of contagious yawning varied substantially across participants, consistent with previous reports [12, 26].
 
Giganti and Zilli [6] studied spontaneous and contagious yawning throughout the course of the day in young adults and reported a mean yawn frequency of 3.09 over a 3 min 12 s yawn-viewing condition conducted at 9:00 am. (i.e., a rate of approximately 0.96 yawns/min). The present study, which was conducted at approximately the same time of day, yielded a lower yawn rate of 0.37 yawns/min during the yawn-viewing condition. This discrepancy may be attributed to the fact that participants in the present study were naġ¨ve to the fact that the study was examining yawning, whereas the subjects in Giganti and Zilli [6] were made aware of the study objectives. Thinking about yawning has been shown to elicit contagious yawning [3, 10]. In the present study, yawn frequency increased not only during the viewing of the yawn video but also during the subsequent rest and gape-viewing conditions. Previous research has suggested that contagious yawning is not a short-latency response but rather is evoked up to several minutes following the presentation of a yawning contagion [3]. Thus, it is possible that the increased occurrence of yawning during the rest and gape-viewing conditions in the current study reflects a carry-forward attribute of contagious yawning. Temporal Coupling of Yawning and Swallowing The present study provides the first evidence of a temporal coupling between yawning and rest swallowing. Sixty-five percent of yawns were followed by a swallow within 10 s and swallows accounted for 26 % of all behaviors produced during this post-yawn period. Moreover, there was a greater-than-fivefold increase in mean swallowing rate during the 10 s following yawning compared with the remainder of the study period.
 
We are aware of only one previous study that examined motor behavior immediately following yawning. Baenninger et al. [9] showed that wrist motion increased during the 15-min period immediately following yawning in young adults, suggesting that yawning is predictive of an increase in motor activity level. Increased skin conductance following yawning also has been interpreted as supporting a period of heightened arousal following yawning [10]. However, decreased skin conductance during the immediate post-yawn period also been reported, making interpretation of these findings difficult [27]. The present study examined a much shorter post-yawn period than Baenninger et al. [9] (i.e., 10 s vs. 15 min) and thus provides novel evidence of a tight temporal association between yawning and subsequent motor behavior, in this case, motor activity involving the upper aerodigestive tract. As to the question of why the participants in the present study tended to swallow immediately after yawning, it is possible that yawning is associated with increased salivary flow which, in turn, evokes swallowing. Salivary flow has been shown to have a direct influence on the rate of resting swallowing in awake adults [28, 29]. While increased salivary flow in association with yawning has been noted briefly in the literature [30], we are not aware of any experimental evidence supporting this claim. Nevertheless, yawn-related modulation of salivary flow seems plausible given that salivary secretion is influenced by both oral sensory stimulation and oral motor behavior [31, 32].
 
For example, increased parotid gland salivary secretion during mastication has been attributed to multiple oral mechanoreceptive inputs from the periodontal ligament, gingival tissues, and possibly the tongue [33]. Yawning produces a pronounced stretching of the orofacial musculoskeletal system that would be expected to stimulate multiple sensory receptors along the length of the respiratory tract, as well as potentially increasing perfusion to muscles surrounding the salivary glands and physically deforming the salivary glands themselves. These yawn-related sensory and motor events may modulate salivary flow.
 
Beyond its potential effects on salivary flow, yawnrelated sensory stimulation of the upper aerodigestive tract may have more direct facilitatory influences on swallowing elicitation. Sensory input is vital to the initiation and regulation of the oral, pharyngeal and esophageal phases of swallowing [34, 35]. This view is supported by evidence that the swallowing motor pattern is (i) modulated by properties of the ingested material and the bolus being swallowed [36&endash;38], and (ii) impaired following oropharyngeal anesthesia [39]. Moreover, it appears that swallowing is elicited not only in relation to the presence of saliva or ingested food and drink; non-nutritive stimuli, including air-pulse trains directed toward the oropharynx [23&endash;25, 40], and electrical stimulation of the oropharynx [41], also have been shown to be associated with increased rest swallowing. As such, it is conceivable that the robust sensory stimulation of the airway produced by yawning exerts an excitatory influence on swallowing regulatory mechanisms.
 
Swallowing is controlled by a medullary central pattern generator that receives cranial nerve afferent inputs and descending inputs from cortical and subcortical centers, and produces a stereotyped pattern of swallow-related contraction of muscles innervated by cranial and cervical nerves [42&endash;44]. This neural network is extensively interconnected with neural circuits that mediate other upper airway behaviors such as mastication and respiration [21, 22]. Moreover, there is evidence that these behavior-specific networks may share neural elements that are functionally recruited to regulate, for example, respiration or swallowing, depending on the prevailing environmental demands [45]. These mechanisms are believed to underlie the precise coordination of respiration and swallowing, and mastication and swallowing, which has been documented through behavioral and electrophysiological studies in a number of species [46&endash;48]. Yawning, like swallowing, is controlled by a distributed network of supratentorial and brainstem regions, including several cranial nerve nuclei [1, 18&endash;20]. The circuits that mediate yawning and swallowing appear to share several brainstem elements, suggesting a neural substrate for the coordination of yawning and swallowing. The present finding of a temporal coupling between contagious yawning and resting swallowing provides behavioral evidence for such a coordinative functional organization.
 
It is possible that yawning influences swallowing indirectly as a result of its effects on respiration and ventilation. Nevertheless, the hypothesis that yawning equilibrates blood O2 and CO2 levels, which has been cited frequently, lacks empirical evidence. The most direct examination of the issue appears to be a study by Provine et al. [49] who studied the effect on yawning of breathing room air (baseline), compressed air (control), 100 % O2, and gas mixtures with higher than normal levels of CO2 (3 or 5 % CO2) in healthy young adults. They reported that breathing CO2 or O2 had no significant effects on yawning rate or yawn duration compared with breathing room air or compressed air. In a companion study, the same authors examined the effects of exercise on yawning rate. While exercise significantly increased breathing rate, it had no significant effect on yawning rate. Another perspective is that yawning may be a mechanism through which the chest wall and diaphragm are adjusted for optimal rest breathing, based on sensory feedback. While our literature review failed to identify any studies that have examined the chest wall and diaphragm correlates of yawning, the current finding of a temporal relationship between yawn and swallow motivates future studies of the relationships between respiratory kinematics and gas exchange, yawning, and swallowing. Adaptive Significance of Post-yawn Swallowing The present finding that swallowing occurs immediately following yawning begs the question: what is the adaptive role of post-yawn swallowing? Swallowing serves both supportive and protective roles, transporting oral contents and ingested material from the mouth to the stomach and protecting the airway from tracheal aspiration [21]. The frequency of both spontaneous and contagious yawning peaks in the early morning and late evening and is correlated with the daily time-course of sleepiness [6, 50]. The early morning and late evening are periods in the diurnal cycle when there would appear to be a premium on ensuring that oral, pharyngeal and laryngeal contents, including whole saliva, nasal and pharyngeal mucus, food stasis, and other particulate matter are cleared from the upper aerodigestive tract. Accumulated debris present in the oral cavity upon waking must be cleared to optimize oral hygiene and prepare the oral cavity for daily activities including eating and speech production. Prior to sleep, there may be an adaptive advantage to clearing the oral cavity of contents to avoid tracheal aspiration during sleep. Additionally, swallow-related esophageal peristalsis would contribute to clearing the bolus from the esophagus prior to lying recumbent, thus reducing the risk of gastroesophageal reflux during sleep [21].
 
Clinical Significance
 
Our finding that yawning and rest swallowing were temporally associated suggests that yawning may hold potential in swallowing rehabilitation, particularly for individuals whose dysphagia is characterized by reduced swallowing rates [51]. Yawn contagions, for example, video images of yawning, could be provided to patients to evoke contagious yawning and, by association, swallowing. This general approach would appear to have several clinical advantages: the stimuli are non-invasive, can be developed readily, at low cost, and accessed through smartphone or tablet technology. In addition, because the patient is not required to follow instructions or perform voluntary movements, the approach might be appropriate for individuals whose rehabilitation options are limited by reduced cognitive and language capacities. That said, carryover of the yawn stimuli to the swallowing of food/liquid has never been examined and would need to be a focus of future research before use of this technique with patients.
 
Conclusion
 
The present study provides preliminary evidence that adult humans tend to swallow immediately following yawning. This finding of a tight temporal coupling between yawning and swallowing suggests that yawning and swallowing are physiologically related, thus extending current models of upper airway physiology and neurophysiology. Our finding also implicates yawning in the elicitation of rest swallowing, a function not considered in previous theories of yawning. Finally, the present demonstration of a temporal association between yawning and swallowing prompts a reexamination of the longstanding question, ''Why do we yawn?''.