resolutionmini

 

 

 

haut de page

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

haut de page

 

 

 

mise à jour du
28 novembre 2002
PNAS
2001; 98; 24; 13995-13999
lexique
Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex
M Iacoboni, LM Koski, M Brass, H Bekkering, RP Woods, MC Dubeau, JC Mazziotta, G Rizzolatti
Instituto di fisiologia Universita di Parma, Via Volturno 39, 43100 Parma, Italy
télécharger l'intégralité de ce texte au format pdf
Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action sur le site Nature Reviews Neuroscience
Resonance behaviors and mirror neurons G Rizzolatti, V Gallese
Cortical mechanisms of human imitation M Iacoboni, G Rizzotatti
The perception-behavior expressway:automatic effects of social perception on social behavior

Chat-logomini

"No creature not endowed with divinatory power can perform an act voluntarily for the first time". Voluntary movements must be preceded, as William James wrote, by "random, automatic, or reflex movements." These movements leave a trace formed by kinesthetic impressions and by their outcome as perceived by the agent of the action ["remote effects"]. The idea of an internal sensory copy of the executed action that in modern time has been reproposed in computer science [forward internal models ] and in psychology [ideomotor theory of learning ] has far reaching consequences for understanding imitation. If the motor representation of a voluntary action indeed evokes an internal sensory representation of its consequences, imitation can be achieved by a mechanism relating this representation with the visual representation of the movement to be imitated and a subsequent re-activation of the relevant motor representations. Evidence that the observed actions are mapped directly onto neurons coding actions has been provided recently by Rizzolatti and coworkers. They demonstrated that in the ventral premotor cortex [area F5 ] and in the parietal area PFk of the monkey there are neurons that discharge both when the monkey makes a specific hand action and when it observes another individual making a similar action (mirror neurons). The issue, however, of whether there is a visual area that codes the observed actions as well as the remote effects of voluntary movements is open. Given its reciprocal connections with parietal area PF (and indirectly with F5), the superior temporal sulcus (STS) region, a cortical sector in which there is a large number of neurons responding to the observation of biological actions, is one of the most likely candidates.
 
The mirror system, given its observation execution matching properties, very likely represents the evolutionary precursor of the human mechanism for imitation, a behavior fundamental for culture transmission. Evidence in favor of this hypothesis was provided recently by an experiment in which we studie dimitative behavior by using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Our reasoning was the following: because mirror neurons are first of all motor neurons, a"mirror area" should beactivated during execution of finger hand movements regard lessof how the movement is actually triggered. Moreover, given that mirror neurons, unlike other cortical motor neurons, are triggeredspecifically by action observation, mirror areas should show an additional activation during imitation, compared with a control motor task. Finally, mirror areas should be activated by simple observation of the action. Two areas with these characteristics were found: area 44 and the rostral most part of the superior parietal cortex. Note that in terms of comparative neuroanatomy, area F5, the area showing mirror properties in the macaque brain, corresponds to area 44 of the human brain. Mirror properties appeared to be present also in a third areal ocated in the STS, thus anatomically compatible with the STS region of the macaque brain, as we reported preliminarily in abstract form.
 
This finding is rather surprising because unlike the first two areas, which are located in cortical sectors where movement-related activity is a characterizing functional property,this third area was located in the cortex mainly dominatedby sensory processing . Also, the activation was only marginally significant and given its unexpected location, additional empirical evidence on its functional properties was needed.We therefore performed a new experiment on a new group of volunteers by using the previously observed area as a search region of interest and instructing the subjects to observe and imitate both left and right hand movements. There is evidence rom psychological studies that humans tend to imitate preferentially mirror-image movements (A common experienceis that when a person touches his right cheek with his righthand, telling another person that there is something on her cheek, the other person touches the left cheek, not the right cheek, with the left hand.) This behavioral evidence suggests a similar privileged neural link between opposite-side effectors. Thus, when using the right hand to imitate, observed left hand actions should produce a stronger activation of the area in which visual information and reafferent copy of the imitated action interact than observed right hand actions. The results corroborated this prediction, suggesting that this newly identified region in the human STS has all the requisites for being a region in which interactions occur between observed action and the reafferent motor-related copy of that action. Both the first experiment that allowed us to identify the region of interest and the second experiment in which we tested the reafferent-copy hypothesis are reported here.
 
Discussion The STS-activated area reported in the present study appears to correspond in its location to the monkey STS region. As shown by Perrett et al. this region is characterized by a large number of neurons that selectively respond to the observation of biological stimuli. Previous imaging studies in which volunteers observed actions such as hand or eye movements also showed that biological moving stimuli activate the human STS region.
 
It therefore appears that both in humans and monkeys, the cortex around the STS is a visual region involved in the analysis of complex biological stimuli. Given these findings, it is not surprising that in the present experiments activation was found in STS during the observation of finger movements. In previous studies in humans the STSregion was observed as activated by biological motion in left,right, or both hemispheres. This difference in the laterality of activation of STS is likely caused by the type of biological actions used as stimuli.
Left hemisphere activation was reported frequently in the case of object-oriented actions. In our experiments the observed action was an intransitive action and required, presumably, a more fine-grained spatial processing, hence the right hemisphere prevalence. Regard less of the activation side, however, what is particularly interesting in our findings is that STS was activated during the execution of finger movements and that this activation was highest when there was a matching between the action that was prepared and the action that was observed.There is general agreement that the temporal lobe processes visual information to give a semantic description of the external word. According to this view, the temporal lobe is the place where the "what" of a visual object is coded, as distinct from the "pragmatic" analysis of the "where" and "how" performed in the parietal lobe.
 
A similar semantic role may be postulated for the STS region but with a specialization for biologically relevant stimuli including body and body-part movements. If this general distinction between temporal and parietal lobe functions is accepted, then the activation of the temporal lobe during action execution can hardly be interpreted as a command to move or, more generally, as an activation causally related to action. Similarly, it is difficult to postulate that the STS activation may represent an intention to move, as it has been suggested forsome sectors of the posterior parietal lobe. It seems much more likely that the STS activation reported here representsa reflection of motor-related activity occurring in the frontoparietal circuits during action execution. The possible anatomical circuitry subserving this functional mechanism may be the connections from the inferior parietal lobe to STS.It is interesting to note that the STS activation of the present study appears to be functionally different from the classical corollary discharges, the aim of which is typically that of canceling or modifying sensory information to maintain stableperception. On the contrary, the present data indicate that the activation in STS is maximal during imitation, i.e., in the condition under which there is a congruency between the observed action and the action to be executed. In other words,the visual representation of action coded in STS is potentiated during action execution, not canceled. This potentiation is notlikely to be caused by unspecific attentional mechanisms.
 
Attentional demands are generally higher for less "natural" tasks.Behavioral studies have demonstrated that in the case of imitation of hand movements, the movement that is imitated naturally is that of the hand of the actor facing the hand used by the imitator. That is, the motor activity evoked by the observation of left hand movement produces a tendency to move the right hand and vice versa. These considerations predict that an increase in attention is more likely to occur when subjects imitate in the less natural condition, which is the opposite of what we observed.It is likely that the phenomenon of imitating in a mirror-like fashion occurs for a natural tendency to interact with other people by using a sector of space common to both actor and imitator. In contrast, there is no reason for this tendency to be present when the observer simply looks at another individual. Exactly this dissociation was found in the STS area reported in the present study.
 
During observation tasks the activity in STS was greater when the right hand was the visual stimulus, comparedwith the left hand. During imitation, the activity in the STS area was greater when the imitators observed the hand mirror image of the hand they used (left hand as visual stimulus and right hand as motor effector). This reversal is likely caused by a modulatory role of the imitative behavior on STS visual activity that, in the absence of imitation requirements, reflects animplicit categorization of the moving hand as referred to thebody of the observer. Although the hand used by subjects to imitate the actions is ipsilateral to the STS region reported here,the motor control at the parietal and premotor level is largelybilateral even for distal movements. It is interesting to note that in our previous report on imitation we described a left inferior frontal area and a right posterior parietal area as endowed with mirror properties.
 
At slightly lower statistical thresholds, however, we observed mirror-like activations also inthe right inferior frontal and left posterior parietal cortex.What we believe happens between the STS, inferior frontal, and posterior parietal cortices in terms of information processingis that STS neurons provide an early description of the action to parietal mirror neurons. These neurons add additional somatosensory information to the movement to be imitated. This more complex information is sent to the inferior frontal cortex,which in turn codes the goal of the action to be imitated. Sensory copies of the imitated actions are then sent back to the STS area for monitoring purposes ("my actions are like the actions I haveseen").
 
In conclusion, returning to the James proposal that movements leave a trace formed not only by kinesthetic impression but also by their visual effects, our data indicate that this functional mechanism indeed may occur in the STS region. During action execution, and in particular during action imitation,the visual representation of biological motion located in STS is activated, and this activation has precisely those properties that an imitation mechanism must posses. It codes actions made by others and stores the remote effects of the movements made by the imitator.
 
Resonance behaviors and mirror neurons G Rizzolatti, V Gallese
Cortical Systems for the Recognition of Emotion in Facial Expressions
Human Facial Expressions as Adaptations: Evolutionary Questions in Facial Expression Research
Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action FindMe
From the perception of action to the understanding of intentionFindMe
Imitation and the definition of a meme Susan Blackmore
Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates Meltzoff AN, Moore MK
Rational imitation in preverbal infants G Gergely
Imitation et agentivité J Proust (CNRS)